FULL TEXT - Canadian Centre for Knowledge Mobilisation
Transcription
FULL TEXT - Canadian Centre for Knowledge Mobilisation
Volume 30, No. 1 Young Children’s Responses to Maximum Performance Tasks: Preliminary Data and Recommendations The Phonetic Notation System of Melville Bell and its Role in the History of Phonetics Introducing a Speech-Language Pathology Outcomes Measure in Manitoba Predicting Reading Abilities from Oral Language Skills: A Critical Review of the Literature Published by the Canadian Association of Speech-Language Pathologists and Audiologists Publiée par l'Association canadienne des orthophonistes et audiologistes Spring Printemps 2006 Southern California, Los Angeles County SPEECH - LANGUAGE PATHOLOGI STS Alhambra Unified School District has an average daily enrollment of 20,000 Kindergarten through grade 12 students. Eight miles northeast of Los Angeles, Alhambra stands at the gateway to the San Gabriel Valley. Within one and one-half hours driving distance from the mountains to the north, the Pacific Ocean to the west, the Mojave desert to the east, and the Mexican border to the south. We are proud of our 13 elementary schools having received the honor of “Distinguished School”. Our five high schools are eligible for the honor this year. We are seeking speech-language pathologists that possess Canadian certification equivalent to a California Clinical Rehab Credential or the California credential. Must be eligible for visa. Our benefits package includes District-paid medical for employee and eligible dependents; District-paid life insurance in a $50,000 group term policy with an additional $50,000 for accidental death or dismemberment; District-paid dental for employee with dependent coverage available; District-paid vision insurance for employee. Salary is $46,350-$75,001 annually (depending on training and experience) Contact the Human Resources Office by calling, writing, or emailing Carla Glenn, Certificated Human Resources Analyst, at the address and phone number below. Email address is [email protected]. Applications are also available on our website. Human Resources Division Laura Tellez, Assistant Superintendent, Human Resources 15 West Alhambra Road Alhambra, CA 91801 (626) 308-2231 (626) 308-1762 (Fax) www.alhambra.k12.ca.us JOURNAL OF SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY AND AUDIOLOGY Purpose and Scope The Canadian Association of Speech-Language Pathologists and Audiologists (CASLPA) is the recognized national professional association of speech-language pathologists and audiologists in Canada. The association was founded in 1964, incorporated under federal charter in 1975 and is committed to fostering the highest quality of service to communicatively impaired individuals and members of their families. It began its periodical publications program in 1973. Indexing JSLPA is indexed by: • CINAHL - Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Health Literature • CSA - Cambridge Scientific Abstracts Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts • Elsevier Bibliographic Databases • ERIC Clearinghouse on Disabilities and Gifted Education • PsycInfo JSLPA Reviewers The purpose of the Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology (JSLPA) is to disseminate contemporary knowledge pertaining to normal human communication and related disorders of communication that influence speech, language, and hearing processes. The scope of the Journal is broadly defined so as to provide the most inclusive venue for work in human communication and its disorders. JSLPA publishes both applied and basic research, reports of clinical and laboratory inquiry, as well as educational articles related to normal and disordered speech, language, and hearing in all age groups. Classes of manuscripts suitable for publication consideration in JSLPA include tutorials, traditional research or review articles, clinical, field, and brief reports, research notes, and letters to the editor (see Information to Contributors). JSLPA seeks to publish articles that reflect the broad range of interests in speechlanguage pathology and audiology, speech sciences, hearing science, and that of related professions. The Journal also publishes book reviews, as well as independent reviews of commercially available clinical materials and resources. Subscriptions/Advertising Nonmember and institution subscriptions are available. For a subscription order form, including orders of individual issues, please contact: CASLPA, 200 Elgin Street, Suite 401, Ottawa, Ontario K2P 1L5. Tel.: (800) 259-8519, (613) 567-9968; Fax: (613) 567-2859; E-mail: [email protected] Internet: www.caslpa.ca/english/resources/ jslpasubscriptions.asp. All inquiries concerning the placement of advertisements in JSLPA should be directed to [email protected]. The contents of all material and advertisements which appear in JSLPA are not necessarily endorsed by the Canadian Association of Speech-Language Pathologists and Audiologists. Lisa Avery, Barbara Bernhardt, Sandi Bojm, Carol Boliek, V.J. Boucher, Tim Bressman, David Brown, Melanie Campbell, Arlene Carson, Margaret Cheesman, Gloria ChiFishman, Patricia Cleave, Pierre Cormier, Luc de Nil, Margaret Dohan, Philip Doyle, Christopher Dromey, Wendy Duke, Andrée Durieux-Smith, Diane Frome Loeb, JeanPierre Gagné, Bryan Gick, Ralph Gilbert, Luigi Girolametto, Carla Hanak, Elizabeth Haynes, Steve Heath, Lynne Hewitt, Jonathan Irish, Marlene Jacobson, Mary Beth Jennings, Andrew Johnson, Benoît Jutras, Elizabeth Kay-Rainingbird, Michael Kiefte, Inge Kirchberger, Robert Kroll, Guylaine Le Dorze, Tony Leroux, Nonie Lesaux, Ian MacKay, Heather MacLean, Heather Maessen, Angela Mandich, Virginia Martin, Deborah Maund, Alison McVittie, Barbara Meissner Fishbein, George Mencher, Kathy Meyer, Robert Mullen, Elena Nicoladis, Greg Noel, J.B. Orange, Johanne Paradis, Carole Peterson, Michel Picard, Kathy Pichora-Fuller, Karen Pollock, Moneca Price, Jana Rieger, Danielle Ripich, Kathryn Ritter, Elizabeth Rochon, Todd Rogers, Christine Santilli, Susan Scollie, Richard Seewald, Barbara Shadden, Ravi Sockalingham, David Stapells, Catriona Steele, Nancy Thomas-Stonell, Anne van Kleeck, Ted Venema, Susan Wagner, Linda Walsh, Jian Wang, Genese Warr Leeper, Penny Webster, Richard Welland, S. P. Whiteside, Connie Zalmanowitz Vol. 30, No. 1 Spring 2006 Editor Phyllis Schneider, PhD University of Alberta Managing Editor/Layout Judith Gallant Manager of Communications Angie D’Aoust Associate Editors Marilyn Kertoy University of Western Ontario (Language, English submissions) Tim Bressmann University of Toronto (Speech, English submissions) Rachel Caissie Dalhousie University (Audiology, English submissions) Patricia Roberts, PhD University of Ottawa (Speech & Language, French submissions) Tony Leroux, PhD Université de Montréal (Audiology, French submissions) Assistant Editor Vacant (Material & Resource Reviews) Assistant Editor Vacant (Book Reviews) Cover illustration Andrew Young Review of translation Tony Leroux, PhD Université de Montréal Translation Smartcom Inc. ISSN 0848-1970 Canada Post Publications Mail # 40036109 JSLPA is published quarterly by the Canadian Association of Speech-Language Pathologists and Audiologists (CASLPA). Publications Agreement Number: # 40036109. Return undeliverable Canadian addresses to: CASLPA, 200 Elgin Street, Suite 401, Ottawa, Ontario K2P 1L5. Address changes should be sent to CASLPA by e-mail [email protected] or to the above-mentioned address. Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 1 REVUE D’ORTHOPHONIE ET D’AUDIOLOGIE Objet et Portée L’Association canadienne des orthophonistes et audiologistes (ACOA) est l’association professionnelle nationale reconnue des orthophonistes et des audiologistes du Canada. L’Association a été fondée en 1964 et incorporée en vertu de la charte fédérale en 1975. L’Association s’engage à favoriser la meilleure qualité de services aux personnes atteintes de troubles de la communication et à leurs familles. Dans ce but, l’Association entend, entre autres, contribuer au corpus de connaissances dans le domaine des communications humaines et des troubles qui s’y rapportent. L’Association a mis sur pied son programme de publications en 1973. L’objet de la Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie (ROA) est de diffuser des connaissances relatives à la communication humaine et aux troubles de la communication qui influencent la parole, le langage et l’audition. La portée de la Revue est plutôt générale de manière à offrir un véhicule des plus compréhensifs pour la recherche effectuée sur la communication humaine et les troubles qui s’y rapportent. La ROA publie à la fois les ouvrages de recherche appliquée et fondamentale, les comptes rendus de recherche clinique et en laboratoire, ainsi que des articles éducatifs portant sur la parole, le langage et l’audition normaux ou désordonnés pour tous les groupes d’âge. Les catégories de manuscrits susceptibles d’être publiés dans la ROA comprennent les tutoriels, les articles de recherche conventionnelle ou de synthèse, les comptes rendus cliniques, pratiques et sommaires, les notes de recherche, et les courriers des lecteurs (voir Renseignements à l’intention des collaborateurs). La ROA cherche à publier des articles qui reflètent une vaste gamme d’intérêts en orthophonie et en audiologie, en sciences de la parole, en science de l’audition et en diverses professions connexes. La Revue publie également des critiques de livres ainsi que des critiques indépendantes de matériel et de ressources cliniques offerts commercialement. Abonnements/Publicité Vol. 30, No 1 Printemps 2006 Inscription au Répertoire ROA est répertoriée dans: • CINAHL - Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Health Literature • CSA - Cambridge Scientific Abstracts Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts • Elsevier Bibliographic Databases • ERIC Clearinghouse on Disabilities and Gifted Education • PsycInfo Réviseurs de la ROA Lisa Avery, Barbara Bernhardt, Sandi Bojm, Carol Boliek, V.J. Boucher, Tim Bressman, David Brown, Melanie Campbell, Arlene Carson, Margaret Cheesman, Gloria ChiFishman, Patricia Cleave, Pierre Cormier, Luc de Nil, Margaret Dohan, Philip Doyle, Christopher Dromey, Wendy Duke, Andrée Durieux-Smith, Diane Frome Loeb, JeanPierre Gagné, Bryan Gick, Ralph Gilbert, Luigi Girolametto, Carla Hanak, Elizabeth Haynes, Steve Heath, Lynne Hewitt, Jonathan Irish, Marlene Jacobson, Mary Beth Jennings, Andrew Johnson, Benoît Jutras, Elizabeth KayRainingbird, Michael Kiefte, Inge Kirchberger, Robert Kroll, Guylaine Le Dorze, Tony Leroux, Nonie Lesaux, Ian MacKay, Heather MacLean, Heather Maessen, Angela Mandich, Virginia Martin, Deborah Maund, Alison McVittie, Barbara Meissner Fishbein, George Mencher, Kathy Meyer, Robert Mullen, Elena Nicoladis, Greg Noel, J.B. Orange, Johanne Paradis, Carole Peterson, Michel Picard, Kathy PichoraFuller, Karen Pollock, Moneca Price, Jana Rieger, Danielle Ripich, Kathryn Ritter, Elizabeth Rochon, Todd Rogers, Christine Santilli, Susan Scollie, Richard Seewald, Barbara Shadden, Ravi Sockalingham, David Stapells, Catriona Steele, Nancy Thomas-Stonell, Anne van Kleeck, Ted Venema, Susan Wagner, Linda Walsh, Jian Wang, Genese Warr Leeper, Penny Webster, Richard Welland, S. P. Whiteside, Connie Zalmanowitz Les membres de l’ACOA reçoivent la Revue à ce titre. Les non-membres et institutions peuvent s’abonner Les demandes d’abonnement à la ROA ou de copies individuelles doivent être envoyées à: ACOA, 200, rue Elgin, bureau 401, Ottawa (Ontario) K2P 1L5. Tél. : (800) 259-8519, (613) 567-9968; Téléc. : (613) 567-2859 Courriel : [email protected]; Internet : www.caslpa.ca/ francais/resources/jslpa-asp. Toutes les demandes visant à faire paraître de la publicité dans la ROA doivent être adressées au Bureau national. Les articles, éditoriaux et publicités qui paraissent dans la ROA ne sont pas nécessairement avalisés par l’Association canadienne des orthophonistes et audiologistes. REVUE D’ORTHOPHONIE ET D’AUDIOLOGIE Rédactrice en chef Phyllis Schneider, Ph.D. University of Alberta Directrice de la rédaction / mise en page Judith Gallant Directrice des communications Angie Friend Rédacteurs en chef adjoints Marilyn Kertoy University of Western Ontario (Orthophonie, soumissions en anglais) Tim Bressmann University of Toronto (Orthophonie, soumissions en anglais) Rachel Caissie Dalhousie University (Audiologie, soumissions en anglais) Patricia Roberts, Ph.D. Université d’Ottawa (Orthophonie, soumissions en français) Tony Leroux, Ph.D. Université de Montréal (Audiologie, soumissions en français) Rédacteur adjoint Libre (Évaluation des ressources) Rédacteur adjoint Libre (Évaluation des ouvrages écrits) Révision de la traduction Tony Leroux, Ph.D Université de Montréal Illustration (couverture) Andrew Young Traduction Smartcom Inc. ISSN0848-1970 Postes Canada Envoi publications # 40036109 La ROA est publiée quatre fois l’an par l’Association canadienne des orthophonistes et audiologistes (ACOA). Numéro de publication: #40036109. Faire parvenir tous les envois avec adresses canadiennes non reçus au 200, rue Elgin, bureau 401, Ottawa (Ontario) K2P 1L5. Faire parvenir tout changement à l’ACOA au courriel [email protected] ou à l’adresse indiquée ci-dessus. 2 Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 Table of Contents Table des matières From the Editor Spring Issue 4 De la rédactrice en chef Numéro du Printemps 5 Article Young Children’s Responses to Maximum Performance Tasks: Preliminary Data and Recommendations Susan Rvachew, Alyssa Ohberg and Robert Savage 6 Article The Phonetic Notation System of Melville Bell and its Role in the History of Phonetics Judith Felson Duchan 14 Article Article Réactions des jeunes enfants aux tâches de durée maximale d’exécution : données préliminaires et recommandations Susan Rvachew, Megan Hodge et Alyssa Ohberg 6 Article Le système de notation phonétique de Melville Bell et son rôle dans l’histoire de la phonétique Judith Felson Duchan 14 Article * Introducing a Speech-Language Pathology Outcomes Measure in Manitoba Sheri-Lynn Skwarchuk, Mark Robertson and Darlene Devlin 18 *Introduction à la Mesure des résultats en orthophonie du Manitoba Sheri-Lynn Skwarchuk, Mark Robertson et Darlene Devlin 18 Article * Predicting Reading Abilities from Oral Language Skills: A Critical Review of the Literature Elizabeth Ekins and Phyllis Schneider 26 Book Review 46 Article *Prévoir les aptitudes à la lecture à partir des capacités langagières : un compte rendu documentaire Elizabeth Ekins et Phyllis Schneider 26 Évaluation de ressource écrit 46 Resource Review 50 Énoncé de position sur l’utilisation de la télépratique par les orthophonistes et les audiologistes membres de l’ACOA 54 Position Paper on the use of Telepractice for CASLPA Audiologists and Speech-Language Pathologists 51 Renseignements à l’intention des collaborateurs 77 Information for Contributors 75 * The review of these manuscripts was coordinated by Philip C. Doyle, PhD * La révision de ces articles a été coordonnée par Philip C. Doyle, Ph.D. Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 3 From the Editor Spring Issue The first article in this issue provides follow-up information to the special issue on technology that constituted the previous issue of JSLPA. In their contribution to the special issue, Susan Rvachew, Megan Hodge, and Alyssa Ohberg presented a tutorial on obtaining and interpreting Maximum Performance Task (MPT) data from children using the TOCS+™ MPT Recorder© ver. 1 (Hodge & Daniels, 2004). MPT data can assist clinicians in diagnosing motor speech impairment in children. The authors showed how the software can facilitate the recording of children’s responses directly to digital computer files. In the article in the current issue, Susan Rvachew, Alyssa Ohberg, and Robert Savage present and discuss pilot data with 4 to 6-year-old children. Based on these data, the authors make recommendations for using MPT tasks in the diagnosis of dyspraxia and dysarthria in this age group. The second article in this issue also has a connection to a previous issue. Judith Duchan has been studying the history of speech pathology for a number of years. An article by Dr. Duchan was published in Volume 29, issue number 2 on the subject of Alexander Graham Bell’s contributions to the field of “elocutionary teaching” in the development of therapy techniques for use with individuals with communication disorders. In the current issue, Dr. Duchan discusses the contributions of Alexander Melville Bell, Graham Bell’s father. Melville Bell invented a phonetic system that could be used for transcribing the speech sounds of any language. His “Visible Speech” was used by the predecessors of speechlanguage therapists; his system had a lasting impact on phonetics, in particular on the representation of vowel sounds. In our third article, Sheri-Lynn Skwarchuk, Mark Robertson and Darlene Devlin discuss the development of the Manitoba Speech-Language Pathology Outcomes Measure. Beginning with models from several other sites, clinicians across Manitoba collaborated to develop the outcomes measure, which permits the cataloguing and storing of severity and priority rating information on clients receiving intervention in the province. The authors discuss issues that had to be addressed in developing the database and make recommendations to others who may be considering development of such a tool in their own regions. Our fourth article is a critical review of the research literature on language measures that predict literacy. As noted by the authors, identification of language predictors of later literacy skills would be potentially useful in detecting and perhaps avoiding difficulties in academic achievement related to language. The various measures of language skills are discussed along with details of selected research articles. The current issue also includes two book reviews and one resource review. In the first book review, Jeanne Claessen, a clinical educator, discusses Clinical Education in Speech-Language Pathology, by Lindy McAllister & Michelle Lincoln. Our second book review is by Susan Rvachew, who reviews Gail T. Gillon’s book, Phonological awareness: From research to practice. Finally, Lu-Anne McFarlane presents a review of the Pre-Reading Inventory of Phonological Awareness (PIPA), by B. Dodd, S. Crosbie, B. McIntosh, T. Teitzel, and A. Ozanne. Reference: Hodge, M.M. & Daniels, J.D. (2004). TOCS+™ MPT Recorder© ver. 1. [computer software]. University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB Phyllis Schneider Editor [email protected] 4 Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 De la rédactrice en chef Numéro du printemps Le premier article de ce numéro fournit un complément d’information au numéro précédent de la Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie (ROA) consacré à la technologie. Dans leur article de ce numéro spécial, Susan Rvachew, Megan Hodge et Alyssa Ohberg présentaient un tutoriel sur l’obtention et l’interprétation des durées maximales d’exécution chez les enfants à l’aide du logiciel TOCS+™ MPT Recorder© ver. 1 (Hodge et Daniels, 2004). Les données sur les durées maximales d’exécution peuvent aider le diagnostic d’un trouble moteur de la parole chez les enfants. Les auteurs montraient comment le logiciel peut faciliter l’enregistrement des réponses des enfants dans des fichiers informatiques. Dans l’article du présent numéro, Susan Rvachew, Alyssa Ohberg et Robert Savage présentent et examinent les données pilotes obtenues auprès d’enfants de 4 à 6 ans. En se fondant sur ces données, les auteurs formulent des recommandations sur l’utilisation des tâches de durée maximale d’exécution pour diagnostiquer la dyspraxie ou la dysarthrie chez un enfant de ce groupe d’âge. Le deuxième article de ce numéro est également relié à un numéro précédent. Judith Duchan étudie l’histoire de l’orthophonie depuis un certain nombre d’années. Un article de Dr Duchan paru dans le numéro 2 du volume 29 abordait les contributions d’Alexander Graham Bell, dans le domaine de « l’enseignement de l’élocution », à l’élaboration des techniques de rééducation destinées aux personnes ayant des troubles de la communication. Dans ce numéro, Dr Duchan discute des contributions d’Alexander Melville Bell, le père de Graham Bell. Melville Bell a inventé un alphabet phonétique que l’on peut utiliser pour transcrire les phonèmes de n’importe quelle langue. Son « langage visible » a été utilisé par les précurseurs des orthophonistes; son alphabet a eu un effet durable sur la phonétique, notamment sur la représentation des voyelles. Dans notre troisième article, Sheri-Lynn Skwarchuk, Mark Robertson et Darlene Devlin discutent de l’élaboration de la Mesure des résultats en orthophonie du Manitoba. À partir des modèles de plusieurs autres sites, des cliniciens de partout au Manitoba ont collaboré à l’élaboration de la mesure des résultats, qui permet de classer et de stocker des données sur la gravité et le degré de priorité des clients suivis dans la province. Les auteurs débattent des questions qu’il a fallu aborder lors de l’élaboration de la base de données et font des recommandations à ceux qui envisagent de concevoir un tel outil dans leur propre région. Notre quatrième article fait un compte rendu des rapports de recherche sur les mesures des capacités langagières qui permettent de prédire le niveau d’alphabétisme. Comme les auteurs le font remarquer, il pourrait être utile de dégager les indices permettant de prédire les capacités de lecture et d’écriture pour dépister et peut-être éviter les difficultés à scolaires liées au langage. Ils discutent des différentes mesures des capacités langagières présentées dans quelques articles de recherche. Le dernier numéro comprend aussi deux comptes rendus de livre et un compte rendu de ressource. Dans le premier compte rendu de livre, Jeanne Claessen, agente de formation clinique, discute de Clinical Education in SpeechLanguage Pathology, par Lindy McAllister et Michelle Lincoln. Notre second compte rendu de livre est de Susan Rvachew, qui a lu Phonological awareness: From research to practice de Gail T. Gillon. En dernier lieu, Lu-Anne McFarlane présente un compte rendu de Pre-Reading Inventory of Phonological Awareness (PIPA), par B. Dodd, S. Crosbie, B. McIntosh, T. Teitzel et A. Ozanne. Référence: Hodge, M.M. & Daniels, J.D. (2004). TOCS+™ MPT Recorder© ver. 1. [logiciel d’ordinateur]. University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB Phyllis Schneider Rédactrice en chef [email protected] Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 5 Young Children’s Responses Young Children’s Responses to Maximum Performance Tasks: Preliminary Data and Recommendations Réactions des jeunes enfants aux tâches de durée maximale d’exécution : données préliminaires et recommandations Susan Rvachew Alyssa Ohberg Robert Savage Abstract The purpose of this study was to examine the ability of 4- to 6-year-old children with typical speech to perform certain maximum performance tasks, with a view to developing diagnostic criteria for identifying dyspraxia and dysarthria in this age group. Twenty children were asked to prolong [a], [mama], [f], [s], and [z] for as long as they could. They were also asked to repeat the syllables [pa], [ta], and [ka] and the trisyllabic sequence [pataka] as fast they could. The children’s responses to the prolongation tasks were highly variable within and across children. Using traditional elicitation methods, these measurements do not appear to be good potential indicators of dysarthria or dyspraxia in this age group. In contrast, repetition rates were much more stable within and across children. All but one child repeated monosyllables at a rate of at least 3.4 syllables per second. Every child achieved a correct repetition of [pataka] at a rate of at least 3.4 syllables per second. Recommendations for interpreting young children’s performance on these tasks are provided. Abrégé La présente étude porte sur des enfants de 4 à 6 ans dont la parole est typique et examine leur capacité à exécuter des tâches de durée maximale d’exécution dans le but d’élaborer des critères de diagnostic pour la dyspraxie et la dysarthrie chez ce groupe d’âge. Nous avons demandé à vingt enfants d’allonger les séquences [a], [mama], [f], [s] et [z] aussi longtemps qu’ils le pouvaient. Nous leur avons aussi demandé de répéter les syllabes [pa], [ta] et [ka] et la séquence trisyllabique [pataka] aussi rapidement que possible. Les réponses des enfants à la tâche de prolongation ont donné des résultats très variables pour chaque enfant et entre les enfants. À partir des méthodes habituelles de d’évocation, ces mesures ne semblent pas être de bons indicateurs de la dyspraxie ou de la dysarthrie chez ce groupe d’âge. En revanche, la fréquence de répétition était beaucoup plus stable. À l’exception d’un seul enfant, tous ont répété les monosyllabes a une fréquence d’au moins 3,4 syllabes la seconde. Chaque enfant a réussi à répéter correctement [pataka] a une fréquence d’au moins 3,4 syllabes la seconde. L’article formule des recommandations pour interpréter la performance des jeunes enfants à ces tâches. Susan Rvachew Ph.D., S-LP(C) McGill University Montreal, QC Canada Alyssa Ohberg McGill University Montreal, QC Canada Robert Savage McGill University Montreal, QC Canada 6 Key Words: speech sound disorders, speech development, maximum performance tasks S peech-language pathologists are expected to conduct an oral-peripheral examination as a part of their standard assessment procedures, even when the client is a young child (e.g., see Bliele, 2002; Hodson, Sherz, & Strattman, 2002; Miccio, 2002; Tyler & Tolbert, 2002). Textbooks about speech sound disorders include specific instructions for conducting such an examination (Bauman-Waengler, 2004; Bernthal & Bankson, 2004; Creaghead, Newman, & Secord, 1989). Asking the child to prolong certain sounds for as long as possible and to repeat certain syllables as quickly as possible is a central part of this assessment procedure. These tasks are administered in order to identify problems with speech motor function that may contribute to the child’s speech sound disorder. Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 Young Children’s Responses Having administered the procedures, the challenge of interpreting the child’s responses remains. Thoonen, Maassen, Wit, Gabreels, and Schreuder (1996) developed some criteria for diagnosing dysarthria and dyspraxia by integrating information about the child’s responses to maximum performance tasks (MPTs), specifically maximum phonation and fricative durations and repetition rates for single syllables and the standard trisyllabic sequence [pataka]. These criteria were derived from the responses of children aged 6 to 10 years of age, some with normally developing speech and some with clinically diagnosed dyspraxia or dysarthria. Briefly, children with dysarthria were found to produce short phonation durations and slow monosyllabic repetition rates; children with dyspraxia produced slow trisyllabic repetition rates and short fricative durations (the specific criteria are described later in this paper). Later, these criteria were cross-validated with new samples of school-aged children, this time including a sample of children with a developmental phonological disorder with no known motoric component. It was shown that these tasks could be used to identify dysarthria with 89% sensitivity and 100% specificity. In other words, 89% of the children with clinically diagnosed dysarthria were identified as dysarthric on the basis of their responses on the MPTs (sensitivity). Furthermore, none of the children who were not dysarthric by clinical criteria were falsely identified as dysarthric on the basis of their responses to the MPTs (specificity). Dyspraxia was identified from MPT responses with 100% sensitivity and 91% specificity. Overall, diagnostic accuracy was excellent with 95% correct classification of 41 children as presenting with normally developing speech, developmental phonological delay, childhood apraxia of speech, or dysarthria. Of particular interest was the finding that children with a developmental phonological disorder performed these tasks in a qualitatively and quantitatively different manner from children with dysarthria or dyspraxia. Children with dyspraxia were often unable to produce a correct trisyllabic sequence. Children with a developmental phonological disorder were usually able to produce the sequence accurately but only after an unusual number of unsuccessful attempts. Overall, the performance of the children with developmental phonological disorders was intermediate between the dysarthric and dyspraxic groups and the normally developing control group. Although the procedures described by Thoonen et al. (1996, 1999) appear to be very useful for the diagnosis of dyspraxia and dysarthria in school-aged children, these researchers concluded that the criteria that they described could not be validly applied to children younger than the age of 6 years. The purpose of the present study was to examine the normal range of performance on sound prolongation and syllable repetition tasks for a sample of 4- to 6-year-old children with typical speech development with a view to developing criteria that will be valid with younger children. Method Participants The children were recruited from inner-city and suburban daycares and suburban kindergarten classrooms in the Montreal area. These preschool settings had EnglishFrench bilingual programs. Parents were asked to volunteer their children to participate in a comprehensive study of oral language and early literacy development, involving two to three assessment sessions, each lasting approximately 45 minutes. Only those aspects of the assessment procedures and the resulting data that are relevant to the children’s maximum performance task will be described here. A parent questionnaire was used to obtain information about the child’s language background and developmental history and family socio-economic status. The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT; Dunn & Dunn, 1997) and the Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation-Second Edition (GFTA; Goldman & Fristoe, 2000) were used to screen for speech and language delay. Of the 29 children whose parents volunteered them to participate in this study, 23 were selected on the basis of the following criteria: proficient speakers of English; aged 4 to 6 years; receptive vocabulary and articulation skills within the normal range; no known developmental delays; no known sensory disorders such as hearing or visual impairment; no known primary medical or developmental conditions that might impact speech and language development. For the first 5 children to be enrolled in the study, their responses on the MPTs were recorded using a digital minidisk player. These children either did not complete the entire test protocol or did so reluctantly and only with much coaxing and multiple visits to their daycares. Their responses will not be described in this report. At this point in the study, the TOCS+™ MPT Recorder© software was developed and then employed to record the children’s performance, as described below. With the aid of this software, 20 additional children completed the test protocol without any extraordinary effort. Only the results recorded from these 20 children will be described in this report. An additional 4 children were recruited but did not complete the assessment due to scheduling problems. This group of 20 children was comprised of 10 girls and 10 boys with a mean age of 69 months (SD = 7.8). All participants either had English as a native language or were judged to be proficient in English. English proficiency was determined by teacher report, examiner’s impression, and receptive vocabulary skills as measured by the PPVT. All of the children’s mothers had either college diplomas or university degrees with the exception of one mother whose highest level of education was secondary school completion. The children’s mean percentile ranking on the GFTA was 37.60 (SD = 17.92), and their mean standard score on the PPVT was 104 (SD = 12.22). One child scored slightly below normal limits on the GFTA but his only Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 7 Young Children’s Responses speech sound error (dental distortion of /s/ and /z/) was judged to be developmentally acceptable and thus this child was included as a participant. Procedure The standardized tests and the MPTs were administered by the third author (Alyssa Ohberg) during two separate test sessions. These sessions included tests relating to a different study of emergent literacy development and thus each session lasted approximately 45 minutes but the MPTs themselves required only 20 minutes on average. Administering the MPTs. The child was asked to prolong the sounds [a], [mama], [f], [s], and [z] for as long as possible on a single expiration. The child was given one practice trial and three test trials for each of these tasks. Then the child was asked to repeat the syllables [pa], [ta], and [ka] and the syllable sequence [pataka], in each case as fast as possible on a single expiration. The child was given one practice trial and three test trials for each repetition task. For the trisyllabic sequence repetition task only, the child was allowed as many as three additional attempts, as necessary, to obtain an accurate repetition of the sequence. The TOCS+™ MPT Recorder© software was used to administer the assessment protocol and record the child’s responses. As described in detail elsewhere (Rvachew, Hodge, & Ohberg, 2005), this software facilitates the recording of the child’s responses directly to digital .wav files using a computer. The software also ensures standardized administration of the protocol as the instructions are available to the child and clinician on a task-by-task basis. The software provides an auditory and visual prompt to the child to begin producing the desired response for each trial. Measurement of durations and repetition rates. The time needed to measure all of the durations and repetition rates obtained from each child was 10 to 15 minutes. The waveform function of TFR (Avaaz Innovations, Inc.), a speech analysis program, was used to measure the durations of each prolongation of [a], [mama], [f], [s], and [z], using visual inspection of the waveform and the partial playback function to identify and mark the beginning and end of each prolonged sound, the duration of which was provided by the TFR software. For the repetition of single syllables, visual inspection of the waveform and the partial playback function was used to identify an uninterrupted sequence of 10 syllables produced on a single expiration, excluding the first and last syllable produced. The duration of this sequence of 10 syllables was provided by the software and then the examiner calculated the number of repetitions produced per second. For the trisyllable repetitions, the duration of 4 repetitions of [pataka] was measured and then the number of syllables produced per second was calculated. Summary statistics. These measurements were then reduced to a number of summary statistics as follows: Maximum Phonation Duration (MPD) = the mean of the 8 durations of the longest [a] and the longest [mama] prolongation; Maximum Fricative Duration (MFD) = the mean of the longest prolongation of [f], the longest prolongation of [s], and the longest prolongation of [z]; Maximum Repetition Rate for single syllables (MRRmono) = the average repetition rate for the fastest repetition of [pa], the fastest repetition of [ta], and the fastest repetition of [ka]; Maximum Repetition Rate for the trisyllabic sequence (MRRtri) = number of syllables per second produced during the fastest accurate repetition of the sequence [pataka]; Sequence = 1 if the child produced a correct repetition of the sequence and 0 if the child did not succeed in producing a correct sequence; Attempts = the number of additional attempts (beyond the first three) that were required for the child to achieve a correct repetition of the sequence. Interpretation. First, the children’s scores for each summary statistic were interpreted in relation to the criteria for identifying dyspraxia and dysarthria as described by Thoonen et al. (1999) for children aged 6 to 10 years of age. Then, scatter plots of the children’s responses were examined to identify criteria that might be more appropriate to the younger children that were assessed for this study. Results Table 1 shows each participant’s longest durations for each prolongation task, fastest repetition rate for each repetition task, and the final score for each of the summary statistics described above. Prolongation durations were highly variable across children, with MPD ranging from 4.25 to 13.94 and MFD ranging from 4.74 to 13.26 seconds. Repetition rates were less variable across children, with MRRmono ranging from 3.03 to 5.11 syllables per second and MRRtri ranging from 3.42 to 6.74 syllables per second. Table 2 shows that stability within subjects was also greater for repetition rates than for prolongations, although reliability across trials was more than adequate for every task except prolongation of [mama]. As described in the introduction, Thoonen et al. (1996, 1999) validated certain criteria for assigning dysarthria scores of 0, 1, or 2 and dyspraxia scores of 0, 1, or 2. Scores of 0 on either scale indicate an absence of the condition while scores of 2 indicate the presence of the condition in children aged 6 to 10 years of age. Thoonen et al.’s criteria are shown in Table 3. All 11 of the control group children with normally developing speech assessed by Thoonen et al. (1999) received dysarthria scores of 0. All but one of these children also received dyspraxia scores of 0. The one control participant who received a dyspraxia score of 2 received speech therapy at a later age after referral by the classroom teacher. We applied these same criteria to the younger children assessed in this study. The result is shown in Table 3 as the proportion of children meeting the criteria for each possible dysarthria or dyspraxia score. Thoonen et al.’s (1999) criterion for ruling out dysarthria is MRRmono greater than 3.5 syllables per Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 Young Children’s Responses Table 1 M axim um Perform ance Task results obtained from 4-, 5-, and 6-year-old children P# A ge GFTA [a] [mama] MPD [f] [s] [z] MFD [pa] [ta] [ka] (months) MRR MRR mono tri S eq Attempts 27 49 84 9.73 9.75 9.74 3.65 15.13 11.48 10.1 3.79 3.89 3.6 3.76 3.98 1 1 25 56 50 14.8 8.48 11.6 7.96 4.96 6.73 6.55 4.65 4.59 4.1 4.45 4.67 1 0 28 59 73 7.71 6.46 7.09 11.22 12.4 11.88 11.8 4.27 3.88 3.56 3.90 3.99 1 0 29 65 30 13.49 8.7 11.1 11.92 11.83 13.35 12.4 4.19 4.16 3.78 4.04 3.95 1 0 6 66 30 9.52 9.17 9.35 4.86 5.28 7.23 5.79 5.28 4.76 5.29 5.11 6.74 1 1 24 66 37 12.12 10.87 11.5 17.8 6.47 13.83 12.7 5.13 4.83 4.45 4.80 6.08 1 0 26 66 32 6.22 11.62 8.95 7.65 7.3 7.73 7.56 4.33 4.12 3.86 4.10 4.18 1 0 7 68 30 8.35 12.35 10.4 5.03 7.88 16.97 9.96 4.05 4.24 3.97 4.09 4.78 1 0 12 68 37 6.57 8.37 7.47 10.1 4.46 9.31 7.96 4.48 4.3 3.76 4.18 3.72 1 0 11 69 27 7.9 11.14 9.52 8.5 7.98 10.78 9.09 5.2 4.92 4.51 4.88 4.24 1 0 9 71 20 15.93 11.95 13.9 6.85 7.79 8.12 7.59 4.57 4.82 4.01 4.47 3.64 1 0 10 71 27 4.29 4.21 4.25 3.48 5.28 5.47 4.74 3.04 2.97 3.09 3.03 3.42 1 3 19 72 48 4.16 8.23 6.2 2.39 8.41 8.01 6.27 4.65 4.53 4.16 4.45 3.99 1 0 21 72 22 12.02 10.88 11.5 8.42 6.68 8.48 7.86 4.56 3.46 4.55 4.19 3.77 1 1 22 72 22 8.55 6.91 7.73 6.08 6.19 8.53 6.93 4.93 4.93 4.57 4.81 3.82 1 3 13 74 52 6.15 5.96 6.06 5.86 6.68 8.11 6.88 4.89 5.06 4.39 4.78 4.59 1 0 15 75 31 10.91 9.17 10 13.62 12.8 13.36 13.3 4.76 4.23 3.85 4.28 3.49 1 0 20 77 14 5.44 5.74 5.59 10.6 13.09 9.58 11.1 4.79 4.81 3.77 4.46 5.05 1 1 17 79 56 7.56 7.17 7.37 3.11 9.04 11.18 7.78 4.7 4.73 4.59 4.67 4.74 1 0 23 83 30 10.11 8.14 9.13 6.53 6.11 11.51 8.05 4.97 5.65 4.5 5.04 4.77 1 1 M 68.90 37.60 9.08 8.76 8.92 7.78 8.29 10.08 8.72 4.56 4.44 4.12 4.37 4.38 1.00 0.55 SD 7.85 17.92 3.34 2.26 2.44 3.90 3.11 2.86 2.47 0.53 0.61 0.49 0.49 0.85 0.00 0.94 Min 49.00 14.00 4.16 4.21 4.25 2.39 4.46 5.47 4.74 3.04 2.97 3.09 3.03 3.42 1.00 0.00 Max 83.00 84.00 15.93 13.94 17.80 15.13 16.97 13.26 5.28 5.65 5.29 5.11 6.74 1.00 3.00 12.35 Note: GFTA scores expressed as percentile ranks. Longest of 3 trials is shown for [a], [mama], [f], [s], and [z]. Fastest of 3 trials is shown for [pa], [ta], and [ka] repetitions. Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 9 Young Children’s Responses second while a MRRmono less than 3 syllables per second leads directly to a diagnosis of dysarthria. These criteria seem to be appropriate even with these younger normally developing children. All but one of the normally developing 4- to 6-year olds enrolled in this study achieved a MRRmono greater than 3.5 syllables per second and no child produced a MRRmono of less than 3 syllables per second. However, monosyllable repetition rates between 3 and 3.5, when accompanied by MPD of less than 7.5 seconds, also lead to a diagnosis of dysarthria when applying the Thoonen et al. (1999) criteria. The one child whose MRRmono was in the borderline range between 3.0 and 3.5 produced a very short MPD and thus received a dysarthria score of 2. However, inspection of the scatterplot shown in Figure 1 reveals that 35% of these young children failed to achieve a MPD that exceeded 7.5. Therefore, these data confirm previous reports that maximum phonation durations are difficult to obtain from children younger than age 6 years (Kent et al., 1987; Thoonen et al., 1996; 1999). However, the expectation that children should achieve a MRRmono that is greater than 3.4 seems appropriate even for these very young children. Table 2 Within subject stability represented as the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) for each task across 3 trials. Task ICC Prolong [a] .71 Prolong [mama] .47 Prolong [f] .88 Prolong [s] .81 Prolong [z] .84 Repeat [pa] .91 Repeat [ta] .94 Repeat [ka] .83 Repeat [pataka] .93 Note: All ICCs are significantly different from 0 with the p values being equal to or less than .001 in each case, except the ICC =.47 in which case p =.047. Table 3 Proportion of Twenty 4- to 6-year old children m eeting the criteria established by Thoonen et al. (1999) for assigning dysarthria and dyspraxia scores to 6- to 10-year-old children Score Classification Criteria Proportion of Children Dysarthria Scores 0 Not dysarthric MRRmono > 3.5 .95 1 Undefined MRRmono 3.0 <> 3.5 and MPD > 7.5 .00 2 Dysarthric MRRmono < 3.0, or .05 MRRmono 3.0 <> 3.5 and MPD < 7.5 Dyspraxia Scores 10 0 Not dyspraxic MRRtri > 4.4 syllables/second .40 1 Undefined MRRtr 3.4 <> 4.4 syllables/second and MFD > 11 seconds, or MRRtr 3.4 <> 4.4 syllables/second and additional attempts < 3 .15 2 Dyspraxic MRRtri < 3.4 syllables/second, or unable to produce a correct sequence, or fails to meet criteria for scores 0 or 1 .45 Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 Young Children’s Responses Figure 1 Maximum repetition rate for monosyllables (MRRmono) plotted against maximum phonation duration (MPD) for each child. Figure 2 Maximum repetition rate for trisyllables (MRRtri) plotted against maximum fricative duration (MFD) for each child. Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 11 Young Children’s Responses Thoonen et al.’s (1999) criterion for ruling out dyspraxia is MRRtri greater than 4.4 syllables per second. As shown in Table 3, only 40% of this sample of normally developing 4- to 6-year-olds met this criterion. Thoonen et al.’s most straightforward criteria for diagnosing dyspraxia are MRRtri less than 3.4 or a failure to produce any correct repetition of [pataka] within 6 trials. Every child enrolled in this study produced a correct sequence and no child produced it a rate slower than 3.4 syllables per second. However, inspection of the scatterplot in Figure 2 reveals that 60% of the children produced a MRRtri that was within the borderline range from 3.4 to 4.4. Nine of these children with borderline MRRtri results were assigned a dyspraxia score of 2 because they required three additional attempts (2 children) and/or their MFD was less than 11 seconds (9 children). However, considering the total group, only 4 children achieved a MFD that was greater than 11 seconds, as shown in Figure 2. With respect to the dyspraxia scores, these younger children are clearly not achieving the minimum expectations for older children with respect to MRRtri or MFD. Discussion The first conclusion to be drawn is that 4- to 6-yearold children can participate in maximum performance tasks. When using the TOCS+™ MPT Recorder© ver. 1, complete data was obtained from 20 young children within a reasonable time period and without undue effort on the part of the examiner to engage the child in the procedures. The second conclusion, not surprisingly, is that the criteria for diagnosing dysarthria and dyspraxia in children older than 6 years cannot be validly applied to children younger than 6 years. However, it does appear that the criteria could be adjusted to yield valid diagnoses with younger children. With respect to the diagnosis of dysarthria, these children achieved repetition rates for monosyllables that were very similar to those obtained from older children. Specifically, all but one child’s MRRmono was greater than 3.4 and no child produced a MRRmono that was less than 3 syllables per second. These findings are consistent with other normative studies of syllable repetition rates (Robbins, 1987; Williams & Stackhouse, 2000). However, maximum phonation durations were considerably shorter than those obtained by Thoonen et al. (1996; 1999) for older children. The use of the MPD to adjudicate the one borderline MRRmono resulted in this young normally developing child receiving a dysarthria classification. One possible solution would be to modify the criteria and the procedure so that MPD is not obtained from children younger than 6 years or used in the diagnosis of motor speech disorders with this population. The criteria could be adjusted to involve only MRRmono as follows: Assign a dysarthria score of 0 (not dysarthric) if MRRmono is greater than 3.4; assign a dysarthria score of 1 (undefined) 12 if MRRmono is between 3.0 and 3.4; assign a dysarthria score of 2 (dysarthric) if the MRRmono is less than 3.0 syllables per second. These criteria would result in 95% ‘not dysarthric’ and 5% ‘undefined’ diagnoses for the normally developing children described in this report. With respect to the diagnosis of childhood apraxia of speech, only 40% of our younger sample met the expectation for older children of a trisyllabic rate greater than 4.4 syllables per second, yielding an unambiguous classification of ‘not dyspraxic’. However, no child produced a MRRtri less than 3.4 and every child achieved a correct repetition of the sequence [pataka] and thus no children received an unambiguous ‘dyspraxic’ classification. The use of the MFD to make a decision about children achieving a MRRtri between 3.4 and 4.4 was clearly inappropriate since the range of MFD scores was great and only one-fifth of the sample was able to prolong a fricative for longer than 11 seconds. Again, the criteria could be adjusted so that only the MRRtri is taken into account, as follows: Assign dyspraxia score of 0 (not dyspraxic) if MRRtri is greater than 3.4 seconds; assign dyspraxia score of 1 (undefined) if MRRtri is between 3.0 and 3.4; assign dyspraxia score of 2 (dyspraxic) if MRRtri is less than 3 syllables per second. These criteria would result in 100% ‘not dyspraxic’ diagnoses for the normally developing children described in this report. More research is required to cross-validate these recommended criteria with a different and larger sample of 4-to 6-year-old children. Validation of these criteria with children who have clinical diagnoses of dysarthria, childhood apraxia of speech, and phonological disorder of unknown origin is also required. Further research to develop a procedure to obtain valid maximum phonation and maximum fricative durations from young children would also be valuable. It seems unlikely that normally developing preschoolers are physically unable to sustain a vowel or fricative for longer than 4 seconds. However, they do sometimes have difficulty understanding the instruction to do so. They also seem to require more time to learn to consciously control the coordination of respiration and speech production. They may not be sufficiently motivated to sustain a single sound for periods longer than 4 or 5 seconds. The availability of software to provide visual real-time feedback to children about the prolongation performance may help them to learn this task more quickly and motivate them to achieve the goal of sustaining a vowel or fricative for at least the criterion duration. In the meantime, the practice of asking children to quickly repeat monosyllables and trisyllabic sequences is a valuable part of the assessment and diagnostic process for children with suspected speech sound disorders. Even children as young as four can be expected to repeat monosyllables and trisyllables accurately and at rates exceeding 3 syllables per second. Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 Young Children’s Responses Acknowledgments Development of the TOCS+™ MPT Recorder© ver. 1 was supported by a grant to Dr. Megan Hodge from the Canadian Language and Literacy Research Network (www.cllrnet.ca) and uses the Universal Sound Server© software developed for the TOCS+ Project (www.Tocs.plus.ualberta.ca) at the University of Alberta by Tim Young. Readers interested in using the TOCS+™ MPT Recorder© can contact Megan Hodge ([email protected]) to obtain a copy of the software. This study was also supported by a research grant to Dr. Susan Rvachew from the Canadian Language and Literacy Research Network and by a research grant to Dr. Robert Savage from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council and by a studentship awarded to Alyssa Ohberg by the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada. This study is one component of Alyssa Ohberg’s master’s thesis research. Author Note Address correspondence to Dr. Susan Rvachew, School of Communication Sciences and Disorders, McGill University, 1266 Pine Avenue West, Montréal, Québec H3G 1A8. References Bauman-Waengler, J. (2004). Articulatory and phonological impairments: A clinical focus. (2nd Ed.). Boston: Pearson Educational, Inc. Bernthal, J. E., & Bankson, N. W. (2004). Articulation and phonological disorders. (5th Ed.). Boston: Pearson Educational, Inc. Bliele, K. (2002). Evaluating articulation and phonological disorders when the clock is running. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 11, 243-249. Creaghead, N. A., Newman, P. W., & Secord, S. A. (1989). Assessment and remediation of articulatory and phonological disorders (2nd Ed.). New York, NY: Macmillan Publishing Company. Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (3rd Ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. Goldman, R., & Fristoe, M. (2000). Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation 2. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. Hodson, B. W., Sherz, J. A., & Strattman, K. H. (2002). Evaluating communicative abilities of a highly unintelligible preschooler. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 11, 236-242. Miccio, A. W. (2002). Clinical problem solving: Assessment of phonological disorders. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 11, 221-229. Robbins, J. & Klee, T. (1987). Clinical assessment of oropharyngeal motor development in young children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 52, 271277. Rvachew, S., Hodge, M., & Ohberg, A. (2005). Obtaining and interpreting maximum performance tasks from children: A tutorial. Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology, 29, 146-157. Thoonen, G., Maassen, B., Wit, J., Gabreels, F., & Schreuder, R. (1996). The integrated use of maximum performance tasks in differential diagnostic evaluations among children with motor speech disorders. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 10, 311336. Tyler, A. A., & Tolbert, L. C. (2002). Speech-language assessment in the clinical setting. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 11(3), 215-220. Williams, P., & Stackhouse, J. (2000). Rate, accuracy and consistency: Diadochokinetic performance of young, normally developing children. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 14, 267-293. Manuscript received: November 29, 2004 Accepted: August 8, 2005 Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 13 Phonetic Notation System of M. Bell The Phonetic Notation System of Melville Bell and its Role in the History of Phonetics Le système de notation phonétique de Melville Bell et son rôle dans l’histoire de la phonétique Judith Felson Duchan Abstract Alexander Melville Bell was an inventor, like his son Alexander Graham Bell. In 1867, Melville Bell invented the first universal phonetic alphabetic system, one that he called “Visible Speech.” Visible Speech was also used by elocutionists of the time to teach speech production to the deaf and to people with stuttering and articulatory problems. Some aspects of Melville Bell’s phonetics contribution had a lasting impact on the science of phonetics, especially in its representation of vowels. Other aspects, such as the notation system he used, were lost to posterity. This article argues that Melville Bell’s work can offer a case study, of sorts, to illustrate that one cannot assume today’s practices will be long lasting ones. Abrégé Alexander Melville Bell était inventeur, comme son fils Alexander Graham Bell. En 1867, Melville Bell a inventé le premier alphabet phonétique universel, qu’il a baptisé « Visible Speech » (le langage visible). Les professeurs d’élocution de l’époque ont également utilisé le langage visible pour enseigner la production de sons aux sourds et aux gens ayant des problèmes de bégaiement et d’articulation. Certains volets de la contribution de Melville Bell à la phonétique ont eu un effet durable sur la science de la phonétique, notamment dans sa représentation des voyelles. D’autres volets, tel le système de notation qu’il employait, n’ont pas passé à la postérité. Cet article soutient que les travaux de Melville Bell peuvent servir d’étude de cas, en quelque sorte, pour illustrer le fait qu’on ne peut pas supposer que les pratiques d’aujourd’hui perdureront. Key Words: history, phonetics, visible speech, phonetic alphabet U Judith Felson Duchan State University of New York at Buffalo Buffalo, New York USA 14 nlike psychologists, linguists, or deaf educators, those in the field of speechlanguage pathology have tended to ignore their historical origins. This historical nearsightedness can lead to a lack of appreciation of how much therapeutic practices depend upon the times in which they originate. We can see from the evolution of Melville Bell’s notation system that knowledge and practices that are taken as established today may either be forgotten or have lasting relevance to later generations. Among the first people in Canada to carry out speech therapy practices was Alexander Melville Bell. While he is best known today as the father of Alexander Graham Bell, Melville Bell deserves more recognition for his own invention—a universal phonetic alphabet. Melville called his alphabet “Visible Speech” and designed it to depict sounds from many languages, as well as vocal noises such as coughing and sneezing. As described by an anonymous contemporary of Melville Bell’s in 1865: “The symbols represent the most elementary actions of the organs: put together they produce compounds. A full sneeze, for example, is a complex operation: it comes among what are called inarticulate sounds; but Mr. Bell writes it down, and, for aught we know, could undertake to furnish every member of the house of Commons with a symbol representative of his own particular sneeze, as distinguished from those of all his colleagues” (Cited in A. M. Bell, 1867, p. 29). Melville Bell worked as an elocutionist and as a college lecturer first in Edinburgh, Scotland, then in Brantford and Kingston, Ontario, and finally, in Boston, Massachusetts. He lectured to young college students and provided elocution lessons to public speakers, Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 Phonetic Notation System of M. Bell deaf speakers, and people who had articulation and stuttering difficulties. He, like his son, performed a version of what we know today as speech therapy. Early in his work Melville Bell became interested in phonetics and the physiology of speech. It was around 1850 when he began developing his alphabet that was to be regarded as the first successful attempt to create a universal system of phonetics. Melville Bell’s Phonetic Alphabet Melville Bell’s phonetic notation system, like the phonetic alphabets that preceded and followed it, portrayed articulation in terms of place, manner, and voicing (e.g., Ellis, 1848; Holder, 1669; Sweet, 1877). But Melville Bell did not use traditional orthography from letters of the alphabet. Instead, he used characters that were iconic representations of the activity of the articulators. The chart in Figure 1 was used by Graham Bell to teach how his father’s system portrayed consonant placement (A. G. Bell, 1906, p. 41). The large curved lines on the lower lip and tongue represent the lower articulators (which the Bells saw as the active ones) and the smaller curved lines on the palate and upper lip represent more passive articulators, the contact point for the active articulators. Figure 1 straight lines to represent articulatory closure (which he called “shut”) that produced stops ( l ). Fricatives were depicted through the use of two small semi-circles that look like a backwardly curved capital E ( ). This was in keeping with Melville Bell’s thinking that the air passes through two sides of the mouth for fricatives. The nasal S ) to represent the shape indicator was shaped like an s (S of the uvula, an articulator whose position is associated with nasality. The voice-voiceless distinction in Melville Bell’s phonetic alphabet was portrayed using two symbols: a — ), and an horizontal line for voiced (a closed glottis) (— elliptical circle (an open glottis) or absence of a vertical 0 ). line for voicelessness (0 Other features, such as aspiration ( > ) and trilling ( ), were also indicated in ways that were reminiscent of a salient aspect of physical production of the sound. The full notation system involved combining indicators of place, manner, voicing and modifying features such as aspiration. For example, in Figure 2 the left-most symbol represents “p” and is comprised of a circle closed at the right showing lip involvement, a vertical line showing articulatory closure (stop), and a right bracket showing aspiration. The second symbol in Figure 2 represents a “b”, combining features that Melville described as “lips shut” and voicing. The third symbol is an “f” showing the “lip divided aperture” and the fourth a “v” adding voicing to the features used to represent the “f”. Figure 2 In order to serve as phonetic symbols for speech sounds, the curve representing larger element or active place of articulation for a sound was separated from the drawing. For example, the curve on the lower lip, a semicircle open at the left, was used to depict labials ( ), the curve on the tongue tip was used to represent alveolars ( ), and the curve on the back of the tongue was used to represent velars ( ). The curves depicting the place of articulation were combined with other symbols that show manner and voicing features of sounds. Like those for place, markings for manner and voice were derived from movements involved in articulation. For example, Melville Bell used Melville Bell’s combination of place, manner, and voicing indicators to form single speech sounds is reminiscent of what was later to be dubbed a distinctive feature approach. (See Fromkin & Ladefoged [1981] for a history of distinctive features.) Vowels in Melville Bell’s system are also straight vertical lines with dots strategically placed on the line to signify where in the mouth the tongue is most constricted. . A dot on the right side of the line is a front vowel (| ), and . one on the left side of the line is a back vowel ( |. ). Dots placed at the top of the line are high vowels ( | ); those low on the line are low vowels ( | . ). Recommended Uses for Visible Speech Alphabet Melville and Graham Bell argued in their various publications for a wide variety of uses for Visible Speech (see Table 1). The Bells saw Visible Speech as a means to enhance speaking, reading, and writing for various populations and as a way to aid in learning foreign Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 15 Phonetic Notation System of M. Bell languages. Melville Bell also aimed to have his Visible Speech alphabet used by linguists, phoneticians, and language teachers as a standard for pronunciation of sounds in different languages. Table 1. Uses of Visible Speech and Sample Sources Where the Uses are Described and Elaborated Teaching children to read (A.M. Bell, 1858) Offering science a universal alphabet (A.M. Bell, 1867) Teaching the blind to read (A.M. Bell, 1867) Teaching oratory to preachers, actors, or anyone with normal speech who wants to improve upon it (A.M. Bell, 1868) Providing a written language for the blind (A.G. Bell, 1872) Teaching illiterate adults to read (A.G. Bell, 1872) Teaching speech to children who are deaf (A.G. Bell, 1872) Capturing the sounds of unwritten language (A.G. Bell, 1872) Comparing the phonetic systems of different languages (A.G. Bell, 1872) Improving the speech of children with articulatory disorders and children and adults who stutter (A.M. Bell, 1878) Teacher training (A.M. Bell, 1883) The Visible Speech system was most often used by the Bells to teach speech to those with communication disabilities. Graham Bell concentrated his efforts for many years on devising ways to use his father’s alphabet for teaching oral language to his deaf students in the UK, Canada, and the US. Graham Bell also used it to provide speech therapy to students in his practice who stuttered or had articulatory problems (Duchan, 2005). The Scientific Impact of Melville Bell’s Invention In the mid 19th century, when Melville Bell published his alphabet, many others in Europe and America were working to develop notation systems for depicting pronounced speech. These scientists came to the phonetic enterprise with different backgrounds and interests. Some, such as Samuel Johnson and Noah Webster, were lexicographers with an interest in capturing standard sound pronunciations. Others, such as George Bernard Shaw and Benjamin Franklin, were spelling reformers looking to standardize, rationalize, and simplify spelling practices. Elocutionists such as Andrew Comstock and Melville and Graham Bell came to the study of phonetics through their interest in oratory and speech therapy. Melville Bell’s Visible Speech notation system was seen by his contemporaries as being an advance over other efforts because it was more precise, it captured sounds of 16 all languages, and it offered a conceptual system for understanding vowels. Bell’s classification of vowels into horizontal (high vs. low) and vertical (front vs. back) dimensions was further developed and popularized by Henry Sweet in 1877. The combined work on vowels of the two men came to be called the Bell-Sweet model of vowel production (Catford, 1981) and is still seen as a viable way of representing the articulation of vowels across languages. Melville Bell’s vowel system was to have an impact on the development of the International Phonetics Alphabet, first issued in Paris in 1889 by the International Phonetic Association. The IPA, as it is now called, was an amalgam of different alphabets that existed at the time and was based on the following set of principles (International Phonetic Association, 1999). · There should be a separate sign for each distinctive sound, that is, for each sound which, being used instead of another, in the same language, can change the meaning of the word. · When any sound is found in several languages, the same sign should be used in all. This applies also to very similar shades of sound. · The alphabet should consist as much as possible of the ordinary letters of the Roman alphabet, as few new letters as possible being used. · In assigning values to the Roman letters, international usage should decide. · The new letters should be suggestive of the sounds they represent, by their resemblance to the old ones. · Diacritic marks should be avoided, being trying for the eyes and troublesome to write. It was on the third criterion, having to do with making the letters in the phonetic alphabet look like ordinary letters of the Roman alphabet, that Melville Bell’s Visible Speech alphabet failed. Those hoping to use Melville Bell’s Visible Speech system found it difficult to learn and remember because the notations were unlike anything they had seen previously (Gordon, 1892). It was also difficult because many of the characters looked alike. Therefore, although Melville Bell’s phonetic studies and notation system preceded the development of the IPA, it was eventually forgotten and replaced by a more familiar, transparent, and therefore more learnable alphabet. Melville Bell’s alphabet is seen by today’s phoneticians as being significant historically because it was the first alphabet that allowed them to depict the sounds of speech independent of the choice of particular language or dialect and because it offered a rational system for understanding and depicting vowels (Catford, 1981). Melville Bell’s Visible Speech system is relevant not only for what it successfully contributed to later generations, but for what did not get passed on. In particular, Melville Bell’s effort to create a notation system whose symbols looked liked articulators ended up being too difficult to learn, especially for those unacquainted with the anatomy involved in speech production. Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 Phonetic Notation System of M. Bell Knowing which of Melville’s contributions lasted and which ones did not has significance to us today. It not only helps us learn about the origins of current practices, but it also helps us understand why the tools and practices considered to be the best in one generation of practitioners may not fit the next. Perhaps the most important lesson to be learned from history is humility. References Bell, A. G. (1872). Visible Speech as a means of communication articulation to deafmutes. American Annals of the Deaf and Dumb, 17, 1-21. Bell, A. G. (1906). Lectures upon the mechanism of speech. NY: Funk & Wagnalls. Bell, A. M. (1867). Visible Speech: The science of universal alphabetics, or selfinterpreting physiological letters, for the writing of all languages in one alphabet (Inaugural edition). London: Simpkin, Marshall & Co. Bell, A. M. (1858). Letters and sounds. A nursery and school book. Salem, MA: James P. Burbank. Bell, A. M. (1868). English Visible Speech for the million. Salem MA: James P. Burbank. Bell, A. M. (1878). Principles of speech and vocal physiology and dictionary of sounds. Salem, MA: James P. Burbank. Bell, A. M. (1883). Visible Speech reader, for the nursery and primary school, requiring no preparatory knowledge of Visible Speech on the part of the teacher. Cambridge, MA: M. King. Catford, J. C. (1981). Observations on the recent history of vowel classification. In R. Asher & E. Henderson (Eds.), Towards a history of phonetics (pp. 19-32). Edinburgh: The University Press. Duchan, J. (2005). The phonetically-based speech therapy methods of Alexander Graham Bell. Journal of Speech Language Pathology and Audiology, 29, 70-72. Ellis, A. (1848) The essentials of phonetics. London: Pitman. Fromkin, V. & Ladefoged, P. (1981). Early views of distinctive features. In R. Asher & E. Henderson (Eds.), Towards a history of phonetics (pp. 3-8). Edinburgh, Scotland: Edinburgh University Press. Gordon, J. (1892). Education of deaf children: Evidence of Edward Miner Gallaudet and Alexander Graham Bell, presented to the Royal Commission of the United Kingdom on the condition of the blind, the deaf and dumb, etc. Washington, D. C.: Volta Bureau. Holder, W. (1669) The elements of speech. London: Scholar Press. Facsimile reprint, 1967. International Phonetic Association (1999). Handbook of the International Phonetic Association. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Sweet, H. (1877). A handbook of phonetics. Oxford: Clarendon Press. Author Note Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Judith Duchan, Department of Communicative Disorders and Sciences, State University of New York at Buffalo, 130 Jewett Parkway, Buffalo, NY 14215, [email protected] Received: September 18, 2004 Accepted: May 9, 2005 Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 17 Speech-Language Pathology Outcomes Measures Introducing a Speech-Language Pathology Outcomes Measure in Manitoba Introduction à la Mesure des résultats en orthophonie du Manitoba Sheri-Lynn Skwarchuk Mark Robertson Darlene Devlin Abstract The Manitoba Speech-Language Pathology Outcomes Measure is a standardized severity and priority rating scale contained on a centralized database. The purpose of the database is to catalogue and store information on individuals aged 0-21 years who receive intervention services by participating speech-language pathologists (S-LPs) in the province of Manitoba. The measure was developed and piloted by a team of clinicians to aid in caseload management, to document the prevalence and severity of specific communication disorders, and to facilitate communication across different programs and regions offering services in speech-language pathology in Manitoba. Information regarding the severity level of an individual’s functional communication, variables affecting priority for intervention, service provider(s), and other demographic information is entered onto a secure website. The information and resulting treatment outcome(s) for the goal area(s) of each individual is calculated and displayed on the website. Aggregate information is available on caseloads for individual speech-language pathologists, for school divisions or health regions, and at a provincial level for participating clinicians. The Manitoba Speech-Language Pathology outcomes measure was piloted in 1999/2000 with the caseloads of three regional groups of speech-language pathologists. It was expanded in the 2000/ 2001 academic school year to include most speech-language pathologists in Manitoba. The database is used currently by approximately 141 clinicians and contains outcome information for over 9,300 individuals from across the province. The purpose of this field report is to describe the process of developing the measure in Manitoba. This paper has implications for other jurisdictions developing and implementing other outcomes measures. Abrégé Sheri-Lynn Skwarchuk, Ph.D. University of Winnipeg Winnipeg, MB Canada Mark Robertson, M.S. Manitoba Education, Citizenship and Youth Winnipeg, MB Canada Darlene Devlin, M.S. Interlake Regional Health Authority Gimli, MB Canada La Mesure des résultats en orthophonie du Manitoba définit une échelle normalisée de classement de la gravité et de la priorité des cas et s’appuie sur une base de données. La base de données a pour but de classer et de stocker des renseignements sur des personnes de la naissance à 21 ans qui ont été suivies par un orthophoniste participant dans la province du Manitoba. Une équipe de cliniciens a élaboré et vérifié la mesure afin d’aider la prise en charge des cas, de documenter la prévalence et la gravité de troubles de communication précis et de faciliter la communication entre les responsables de programmes et les régions du Manitoba qui offrent des services d’orthophonie. On verse dans un site Web sécurisé les données relatives à la gravité du trouble de communication fonctionnelle, aux variables ayant un effet sur la priorité d’une intervention, aux fournisseurs de services ainsi que d’autres données démographiques. On compile et affiche sur le site Web les données et les résultats du traitement qui s’ensuit en fonction des objectifs fixés pour chaque personne. Il est possible de consulter des données sur l’ensemble des cas d’un orthophoniste clinicien participant, d’une administration scolaire, d’une région sanitaire et d’une province. La Mesure des résultats en orthophonie du Manitoba a fait l’objet d’un projet pilote en 1999–2000 portant sur les cas de trois groupes régionaux d’orthophonistes. L’essai s’est étendu à la plupart des orthophonistes du Manitoba durant l’année scolaire 2000–2001. Environ 141 cliniciens utilisent actuellement la base de données, qui contient les résultats du suivi de plus de 9 300 personnes dans toute la province. Ce rapport d’utilisation a pour but de décrire le processus d’élaboration de la mesure au Manitoba. Ce travail aura des répercussions sur l’élaboration et la mise en application de mesures des résultats dans d’autres provinces ou territoires. Key Words: outcome, database, severity, priority, functional communication measure 18 Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 Speech-Language Pathology Outcomes Measures Introducing a Speech-Language Pathology Outcomes Measure in Manitoba Given the increased demand for clinical and therapeutic support services in various public jurisdictions (e.g., Proactive Information Services, 1998), clinicians are feeling pressure to provide high-quality services for children and their families in an efficient manner. Complicating clinicians’ responses to increased demands are factors such as service overload, recruitment and retention, and funding. In many cases, resource-limited funding models (Weber, 1994) prevent optimal service delivery for those who require it. Consequently, waiting lists for most publicly-delivered and some privately-delivered clinical services are long. Direct assessment and intervention by a specialist, such as a speechlanguage pathologist (S-LP), are limited, and therapy becomes focused more on intervention than prevention strategies, all of which raises concern over the effectiveness of current service delivery models. As a result, clinicians have been involved in the process of finding solutions to these problems to alleviate workload and caseload stress, and ultimately to enhance service delivery (Schooling, 2000). Research indicates that in addition to concerns over working conditions, due in part to increased caseloads, clinicians are feeling increased pressures to be accountable for the services they provide (Schooling, 2000). This pressure to ensure accountability has been highlighted recently in the field of special education. Accountability is necessary to ensure that individualized programming efforts are effective, to justify the high cost of special needs programming, and to combat the growing number of students referred for service in a resource-limited model (Proactive Information Services, 1998). Furthermore, since specialized educational services in areas such as speech-language pathology are often housed in separate, periphery departments in school divisions, the large budgets needed to sustain these specialized services can become a target for cutbacks in times of fiscal restraint (Weber, 1994). The policies and procedures in place to ensure accountability of teachers, principals, and schools are clear. Curricular documents are available (e.g., Manitoba Education and Training, 1997a, 1998; Western Canadian Protocol, 1996, 1998) and academic standards are documented within them to ensure that students are exposed to certain materials and topics in school. There also are guidelines established for reporting on student progress and achievement (Manitoba Education and Training, 1997b). In most Canadian provincial and territorial jurisdictions, students also complete a locally developed standardized exam at various grade levels (Skwarchuk, 2004) to ensure they are meeting academic expectations (Manitoba Education and Training, 1999). Descriptions of effective teaching strategies are available from provincial education departments such as Manitoba Education and Training (1996). Guidelines for appropriate professional practice are available from professional teaching organizations such as the Manitoba Teachers’ Society (2003). Perhaps due to the specialty and individualized nature of the service provided, there are few publicly documented guidelines and expectations set for the delivery of special education and clinical services, such as the profession of speech-language pathology. Consequently, clinicians are left to their own wisdom and professional expertise to act in the best interest of the individuals they serve. Furthermore, since most of the work in speech-language pathology is conducted at the individual level, S-LPs work to assess and provide programming on a case-by-case basis. The optimal distribution of such services to individuals and the efficient delivery of those services in the most cost-effective manner is yet to be determined. However, in a recent review of the Special Education services in Manitoba conducted by an independent research firm, a recommendation was put forth that the province develop a mechanism for tracking students receiving special services in general, including speechlanguage pathology, and documenting the services provided (Proactive Information Services, 1998). The provincial outcomes measure was not mentioned in the review because it had not yet been developed. One method of addressing increasing concerns over accountability is to document caseload size and measure intervention success by tracking pre- and post-intervention data, and then measuring the outcome(s) or amount of change after a period of intervention (Swigert, 1997). In these designs, it is preferable to have a control group (Cozby, 1993), but in applied research settings, a suitable control group is not always available. This information often has been tabulated in databases developed by professional organizations or governing agencies worldwide that are interested in the caseload makeup of an entire region, the collective effectiveness of the various interventions used in the field, the equitable prioritization of individuals, and the overall job satisfaction and well-being of the service providers (Gallagher, Swigert, & Baum, 1998). For example, the United Kingdom has developed the Therapy Outcome Measure (Enderby, 1997) to provide S-LPs with a “practical tool to measure outcomes of care by providing a quick and simple measure which can be used over time” (John & Enderby, 2000, p. 287). Similarly, the American Speech-LanguageHearing Association has endorsed the National Outcomes Measurement System (American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, 1996, 1997) “to assist its members in the collection of outcome data” (Schooling, 2000, p. 4). Canadian efforts include the Priority Rating Scale (PRS), developed by clinicians in New Brunswick in 1997 (Eval-Plan Consulting, 1998), and a document written in 1993 by Alberta Health, outlining structural standards (i.e., serving the target population effectively), process standards (i.e., competent and acceptable delivery of services) and outcomes standards (i.e., ensuring that program objectives are met, using a priority rating scale) of professional practice. In addition to providing data for accountability purposes, a system for keeping track of pre- and postintervention data would be helpful for clinicians. The data can serve to identify the size of individual caseloads, including the prevalence of frequently and infrequently occurring speech- and language-related disorders within the province for participating clinicians, and as a function of each school Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 19 Speech-Language Pathology Outcomes Measures or region. Data entered would also reveal the percentage of time devoted to direct versus indirect service delivery or group versus individual intervention, and the related effectiveness of the intervention. Furthermore, these data would be helpful in improving clinicians’ understanding of interventions and simplifying report writing. Communication across regions and jurisdictions of S-LPs could be improved if all clinicians used the same data recordkeeping and data-tracking system. Finally, the data collection would be meaningful in terms of explaining the goals and outcomes of programs and the time required to deliver programs effectively, thus globally substantiating the importance of the speech-language pathology profession to the general public. The success of implementing these programs is highly dependent upon marketing the tool effectively. Stakeholders will support outcomes programs because, among other reasons, they address increasing concerns over accountability, they determine best educational practices in speech-language intervention, and they ensure cost-efficient service delivery models (Gallagher, Swigert, & Baum, 1998). Clinicians can be encouraged to support outcomes programs if they are user-friendly, save time and minimize paperwork, provide reliable and valid information, and more globally, can be used in discussions concerning the overall worth of the speech-language pathology profession. These outcome programs also must sustain durability due to changes in political priorities, government cutbacks, and coordination between ministries and/or departments of education and health (Law, Lindsay, Peacey, Gascoigne, Soloff, Radford, & Band, 2001). Consequently, despite their strengths and educational potential, the aforementioned Canadian outcomes projects in both Alberta and New Brunswick were not considered to be educational priorities and have since been discontinued. The reasons for the development of an outcomes measure in several countries and regions are similar to the reasons for developing a measure in Manitoba. Global pressures concerning accountability in education (e.g., Reeves, 2002) and health-related fields, in combination with the increasing demand for services and the associated increase in costs of these perceived ancillary costs to the general public, provide a rationale for the development of such measures. The Manitoba project, however, was not a product of a professional organization or government mandate. The project was unique in that it was initiated by a group of practicing clinicians who were interested in understanding and improving their individual and collective service delivery. The fact that the project met several of the global concerns contributed to its appeal and provided time and resources for the measure to be developed further. The future success of its implementation is dependent upon continuing to establish consensus within the field to design and implement a measure, ensuring that the measure actually aids in alleviating caseload stress, endorsement of the measure from an overarching agency or agencies, strong collaboration among team players to ensure face validity, and establishing procedures to ensure reliability. The purpose of this paper 20 is to describe the process through which the measure was developed. The Manitoba Project The Manitoba Speech-Language Pathology Outcomes Measure project evolved over three phases as a grassroots endeavor. Initial meetings were held to develop a tool for measuring treatment outcomes with a group of S-LPs practicing in the Interlake region of the province. The Interlake region is a rural geographical area north of Winnipeg, between Lake Winnipeg and Lake Manitoba. These meetings were then expanded to include the South Central regional group of S-LPs, another rural group of clinicians practicing south west of Winnipeg, and to provide further expertise and diversity to the working group. In the second phase of the project, S-LPs in the South East region of the province were invited to participate and review the initial draft of the measure. The final phase involved expanding the project to all geographical regions in Manitoba. The process of developing the project with respect to these three phases is described in detail below. Phase 1 — Developing Ideas Given the reported importance of implementing outcomes measures in other provincial and international jurisdictions and the perceived benefits of using such measures, two regional groups of S-LPs met along with Manitoba Education, Citizenship and Youth personnel to develop a measure for use in Manitoba. The working group represented a diverse range of S-LPs working in the education, family services and health care professions. Initial meetings focused on researching available outcomes measures used in Canada and the United States. The group discussed the relative merits of each measurement scale as it would apply to service delivery in Manitoba. Each scale was evaluated in terms of ease of use, comprehensiveness with regard to diverse work settings, populations served, assumptions regarding service delivery models, face validity, and the inherent ability to capture change. Based on this review and clinicians’ field experiences, the working group decided to draft a new measure that: 1) incorporated the combined strengths of the ASHA NOMs, New Brunswick Priority Rating Scale, and the outcomes prescribed by Alberta Health (1993); and 2) focused more positively on functional status rather than impairment. The portions of the reviewed outcome measures that accurately described Manitoba caseloads were incorporated into the outcomes measure. Portions that needed to describe current caseloads more accurately than text that was already available were written by subcommittees, reviewed by the group, and then incorporated into the Manitoba measure. For example, the team felt that the prioritization system used in the Alberta measure was innovative and seemed to capture the variables required in prioritizing individuals receiving speech-language pathology services. Consequently, members combined the Alberta prioritization system with Prognosis and Related Factors elements from the New Brunswick system into a working model for prioritizing caseloads and treatment Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 Speech-Language Pathology Outcomes Measures in Manitoba. The team members also felt the severity ratings of the functional communication measures from the ASHA NOMs were user friendly, had broad applicability, and were relatively concise. Thus, permission was sought from ASHA to adapt the severity ratings for the functional communication measures as described in the ASHA NOMs. However, since the Manitoba team valued a unified system of data entry, coding, and analysis, the ASHA school-aged and preschool NOMs descriptors (which are separate in their outcomes model) were combined to provide the same severity rating scale of the functional communication measures for all individuals aged 0 to 21 years. Manitoba clinicians also incorporated some unique features (i.e., addressing concerns related to augmentative communication, expanding on the description of all severity levels to ensure reliable coding, and incorporating recent developments in the areas of phonological awareness and word finding into the severity rating scale) to capture the essence of a case. Finally, the team reviewed descriptions of each functional communication measure to ensure that each specific level in each of the scales was functionally-based and would be sensitive enough to capture improvements in an individual’s ability to function at home, school, work, or play. Broadly stated, the provincial outcomes measure was designed: · to document the prevalence and severity of specific communication disorders, treatment priorities, and outcomes from the interventions for individuals serviced by S-LPs up to and including the age of 21 years across the province of Manitoba; · to summarize information on caseloads and treatment provisions for individuals receiving speech-language intervention across the province in schools, health regions, and family service and housing regions; and · to facilitate communication across different programs and regions offering services in Manitoba, and to improve consistency in description of service delivery. The working group developed a manual for implementation and a standard form for reporting caseload summaries. This manual has since been revised and a current version is available on the Manitoba Education, Citizenship and Youth (2003a) website: www.edu.gov.mb.ca/ks4/ specedu/slp/manual.html. The current version of The Manitoba Speech-Language Pathology Implementation Manual (2003a) contains information on the developmental history and purposes of the project, instructions on completing the treatment summary form on an individual, descriptions of the 12 functional communication measures (FCMs) used in the outcomes measure, information on scoring the four levels of the severity variable, and the four categories (severity, urgency, related factors, and prognosis) associated with the priority variable. The manual also contains several case study examples, a list of concomitant factors affecting S-LP intervention (e.g., spina bifida), a set of variables that can affect treatment outcomes (e.g., level of support), and a glossary. In the next phase of the project, the same group of regional clinicians evaluated the preliminary measure by piloting it with case studies. Disagreements in coding and ambiguities in the measures were resolved through discussion. Discussions continued until consensus was reached. In some cases, the discussions led to development of new terminology with a standard definition to eliminate individual interpretations of previously used terms. For example, although some clinicians used the terminology “consultative collaborative” to indicate a broad range of S-LP involvement, the term was redefined provincially to refer to cases where the S-LP was involved in the assessment and recommendations of treatment and periodic follow-ups to review program and carry-over goals. Several practice sessions focused on applying the outcomes measure to individuals receiving treatment from actual caseloads in the field and trouble shooting problems. Phase 2 -- Piloting the Project in the Interlake, South Central, and South East Regions of Manitoba Efforts were made to expand the project into another region of Manitoba after a working copy of the manual was completed. The development team wanted to ensure adequate content validity and applicability in the field. The South East region of S-LPs was chosen because it represented another geographically distinct region in Manitoba, the S-LPs in the region were within driving distance of the other group for ease of facilitating meetings, the S-LPs represented a diverse group comprised of both new and experienced clinicians, and the demographics of the individuals receiving the S-LP services were diverse. The new region expanded on the range of ages served and the cultural demographics of individuals served, and represented a variety of service delivery models. The three groups of S-LPs met and the manual was revised to eliminate any concerns. Specifically, several functional communication measures were collapsed or moved to concomitant factors. The revisions were made to ensure that the communication measures were as functional as possible (i.e., they focused on clear descriptions of the individual’s current performance capabilities), they were hierarchical in nature, and they represented areas that were sensitive to change. Several changes were introduced. For example, the functional communication measure for Hearing Sensitivity was removed because it was determined that a functional communication measure based on severity of hearing loss would not be remediated by treatment but would be a significant concomitant factor affecting outcome. As a result, hearing sensitivity was removed as a functional communication measure and Deaf/Hard of Hearing was added as a concomitant factor. The functional communication measure of Central Auditory Processing was also changed to a concomitant factor. Functional communication measures in the areas of Cognitive Orientation, Pragmatics, Language Comprehension, Language Production, and Fluency/Rate/Rhythm were adapted significantly through group input and consensus to make the individual scales easier than the original version for understanding, interpreting, and capturing functional change. Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 21 Speech-Language Pathology Outcomes Measures Clinician data was initially collected on a Treatment Outcome Summary Form and submitted to central support personnel. The computer software program Excel was used to generate caseload outcomes and reports. Due to a perceived increase in the availability of computer services and constant technological upgrades in internet access, the working group decided to mount the outcomes database on a secure website. A website would ensure that the information could be entered and accessed in almost any location and would facilitate data entry. The website was designed to be user friendly and included flip-down windows with options to select required information, thereby minimizing the amount of information to be entered individually. A database developer was hired to develop the website and organize it in ways that would be beneficial to S-LPs, their regional directors, and other governing agencies. Clinicians were asked to access the website and enter data for each individual on their caseload, including identifying information, severity level of functional communication and a rating of variables affecting priority. Support personnel were available to help clinicians with the initial stage of data entry if required. Each clinician entered a password and received access only to his or her own individual caseload. Clinicians assigned each individual a unique identification code to prevent the duplication of individual entries. In this respect, they could access individual case files at the times of assessment and reassessment. The website provided opportunity to review and edit all data, enabling any errors in data entry to be easily changed. Some provisions were created within the database to ensure that only certain characters and certain numbers of characters could be entered to the various data fields, limiting the number of data entry errors. School student service coordinators and regional program directors also could obtain a password to access aggregate data on their school division/facility, including the types of services provided and the treatment outcomes. The website was also organized so that this aggregate information was available on the caseloads of participating clinicians for the purpose of creating summaries of prevalence rates, priority ratings, and treatment outcomes. Phase 3 — Expanding the Project into All Areas of the Province To expand the use of the measure to all areas of the province, the project was described at regional workshops conducted by the provincial S-LP consultant. S-LP participants received a copy of the implementation manual before the session. The workshops focused on accessing and using the website, examining cases to establish reliability in using the codes, and practicing using the manual and website with an actual case. The same government representative presented the measure to all S-LPs to ensure the delivery of consistent information. Addressing Reliability and Validity Issues from an Action-Based Research Model In developing a measurement scale, researchers and educators often are concerned with establishing acceptable 22 levels of reliability and validity (Gay & Airasian, 1996; John & Enderby, 2000). The reliability of a measure is concerned with establishing consistency in data measurement, and good reliability represents less error in data interpretation. The reliability of a measure may be established by obtaining the same score for an individual by using different testing instruments, using different raters or establishing the same rating over a period of time. Validity, on the other hand, is concerned with whether the score measures what it is intended to measure, and whether the assessment is appropriate and meaningful to its users (Gronlund & Cameron, 2004). There are different types of reliability and validity described in the testing literature and formal measures for assessing them. Since the project started at a grassroots level with S-LPs participating from all areas of the province to develop a system of classification that would work for them, there was no initial intent to gather information formally on reliability and validity. A rigorous developmental process was implemented to ensure that reliability and validity issues were established in a general and informal sense. The project operated from a qualitative, action research perspective. Thus, the reliability and validity of the measure was grounded in the clinical expertise of its developers. Under the assumptions of a qualitative research paradigm (e.g., Johnson & Christensen, 2000), the following factors were considered in the discussions of reliability and validity. First, the data were collected over a long period of time (one year of pilot testing and two subsequent years of provincial data collection), involving a large number of individuals (i.e., currently 9,300 individuals) from across the province. Developers made the assumption that any inherent and obvious problems associated with the validity and reliability of the measure would be identified by its users during this pilot phase of the project. Consensus also was established on the terminology to be used, even if it meant redefining certain phrases and keywords to establish continuity within the provincial field. Findings were triangulated by cross-checking information and conclusions through a number of sources (e.g., the working group of clinicians, case examples, and comparisons of scores with those obtained in other outcomes measures). Discussions also were held to review disparate cases and resolve any tentative explanations in the data. These discussions incorporated the perspectives of multiple S-LPs from rural and urban areas who were employed in a variety of settings to ensure that the findings were representative and that the tool made sense to clinicians working in the field. Regular meetings and peer review sessions were held with the initial developmental team and other clinicians across the province to conduct critical reflections and eliminate potential biases that may have affected the developmental process of the tool. Finally, presentation, review, and support for S-LPs using the measure was completed by the same consultant. To ensure ongoing development of the measure, a committee of practicing S-LPs from across the province has been formed. The committee, in response to S-LP input, reviews, revises and expands the current applicability and utility of the measure. Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 Speech-Language Pathology Outcomes Measures In summary, concerns over reliability were minimized informally by having a large number of S-LPs score clinical interpretations from a large number of diverse cases. These clinicians worked with one provincial consultant who then trained all additional S-LPs, to ensure that the training was consistent for SLPs using the measure. In terms of establishing validity, a wide range of S-LPs representing diverse background experiences and working environments were involved in developing the measure and scoring resulting cases. A rigorous review process was used to ensure that scales were meaningful and reflected the collective experiences of S-LPs working in the province. This rigorous developmental process would ensure that future formal studies of reliability and validity would not lead to disappointing results. Positive and Negative Experiences in the Development and Implementation Process The process of developing, organizing, and implementing an outcomes measure for use at a provincial level has been rewarding and challenging. The next section will focus on the positive and negative aspects of developing the measure with grassroots clinicians. Positive Experiences Important for the development of such a measure is the mandate and support from a governing body or institution to facilitate change. The goals and focus of the project aligned with recommendations to improve service delivery in a recent review of Special Education services in Manitoba (Proactive Information Services, 1998). Consequently, it received strong governmental approval and support. In fact, the project was highlighted as an area of priority in a governmental discussion paper released in March of 2001, and it was cited as an area of best educational research practice by Manitoba Education, Citizenship and Youth. From the onset, the project was coordinated by one consultant. This person completed the background research for the project, networked with personnel in other jurisdictions to obtain information on other initiatives, and served as a contact person and supporter for clinicians using the measure. This same contact person was responsible for providing supervisory support to all new school clinicians. The consultant has developed close working relationships with most new clinicians in the province over the last six years. The development of these relationships was beneficial in terms of gaining compliance to try the measure and providing ongoing on-site support, especially for clinicians practicing in remote rural communities and settings. The Manitoba outcomes project was strongly supported by a grassroots effort of clinicians. The project was initiated by a front line S-LP concerned with the need to describe caseloads and intervention outcomes in a functional way for S-LPs and administrators. During the developmental stages, collaboration was required from three governmental department jurisdictions responsible for S-LP intervention. Despite discrepancies in service delivery models and issues that could ensue in times of limited resources, the team worked collaboratively to develop a system that would work for them. Lengthy discussions resulted in group consensus on issues of content, terminology and implementation. Clinicians held discussion groups after hours, tested the various outcomes scales on individuals on their caseload, and assisted in any way possible to facilitate the development of the measure. The appeal to use the measure came from the fact that clinicians could use the results from the database to make decisions, instead of relying on gut feelings. Finally, for the first time ever, an attempt has been made to obtain aggregate data at a regional and provincial level on prevalence rates, service delivery and outcomes based on gender, age, grade, priority variables and severity levels of communication disorders (Manitoba Education and Youth, 2003b). The aggregate data collected to date have been used to plan for some current and future service delivery in speechlanguage pathology. Challenges The developmental team worked to address concerns in three general areas: technological difficulties, clinician acceptance and support, and end-result test score interpretation. These three areas of concern are discussed in more detail below, both in terms of how they were viewed as problems and how they were addressed. When the project was first initiated, clinicians commented that they wanted a user-friendly, easily accessible system. Program developers were hired to design the interface, allowing for the efficient storage and retrieval of the data on the internet. However, due to technological difficulties associated with internet access, speed, and system incompatibility, some clinicians experienced difficulties with this set-up. To preserve confidentiality and anonymity for individuals on their caseload, S-LPs had to be careful when working on a computer system with multiple users such as in a school computer lab. Furthermore, since many people were involved in entering data on the website, incompatible conventions in data entry occurred. Finally, technical difficulties with the database required assistance from the system developer. Although clinicians support the outcomes measure, concerns have been raised over time commitments and associated benefits of a new data collection system. To address these concerns, government personnel have promoted the tool by offering follow-up sessions on data entry short-cuts, profile interpretation, and ways of marketing the measure to employers. Discussions have been held to circumvent any problems that have occurred with on-line data entry. In addition, some changes have been made to improve the storage and retrieval of information contained on the database in order to enhance access to the database for the purposes of conducting additional statistical analyses. It is important to have support consultants available to focus on naturally occurring glitches as soon they are identified. Finally, in the development and use of the Manitoba Speech-Language Pathology Outcomes Measure, clinicians expressed concern over using different standardized tests, making conclusions from those test score results, and then using the outcomes measure to summarize results (e. g., two Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 23 Speech-Language Pathology Outcomes Measures different S-LPs could obtain the same score for an individual based on the administration and interpretation of different tests). To address this problem, some clinicians felt that S-LPs should use the same assessment tools to establish consistency in scoring. Discussions were held and it was decided that these concerns were related to the entire speech-language pathology profession as opposed to the Manitoba SLP Outcomes Measure per se. The developmental team decided not to mandate certain assessments as this inflexibility would undermine the clinical training and expertise of those persons using the measure. In the initial stages of the project, clinician concerns over the potential data to be used in evaluating the individual performance of clinicians was identified and acknowledged by the group as a risk factor in the implementation of the Manitoba measure. The group agreed that comparisons and evaluations of individual clinicians currently occur without the benefit of formal standardized data. It was agreed that clinicians would be empowered by bringing standardized data on the nature of their caseloads and services provided, to the administration and to the general public. Overall, the group felt that the benefits of standardized data and informed decision making outweighed any risk involved. Recommendations and Conclusions During times of budgetary restrictions, clinician shortages, and overextended caseloads, service providers are searching for efficient ways of delivering speech-language pathology services. The Manitoba Speech-Language Pathology Outcomes Measure enables clinicians to document the size and severity level of their caseload, assess the treatment priority of their individuals, and determine the kinds of interventions provided. For the most part, the implementation of the measure has been perceived positively in the field and it is currently being used voluntarily by most pediatric S-LPs in the province. Some agencies mandated to provide evaluation of their programs have chosen to use the Manitoba measure. Despite its success, the project experienced growing pains. Based on our experiences in developing and implementing the measure, we suggest the following recommendations for others involved in creating similar measures: Establishing Connections in the Field. Establishing personal connections with clinicians in the field was essential to facilitating communication about the project. The close connection between the provincial consultant and individual clinicians implementing the measure ensured that problems could be detected early and circumvented. Frequently held regional team meetings also improved the morale and commitment of clinicians to the project by giving them a sense that they were working on a new, innovative project together, and by providing opportunities for problem solving to further develop the measure. Marketing the Tool. Discussions regarding the benefits of developing and implementing an outcomes measure should be held with all stakeholders (i.e., clinicians, regional directors, government staff, and provincial and national 24 professional organizations). For the Manitoba project, these discussions were facilitated by the provincial consultant who traveled to different regions, held dialogues with the involved parties, drafted reports on the progress of the measure, and in general, served as an advocate for the outcomes measure. Developers of the ASHA NOMs scale have made similar recommendations with respect to marketing their measure (Goldberg, 1997). Working with Technological Difficulties. The software for the database should be chosen carefully, considering not only the ease of inputting data and the storage and retrieval of information at different hierarchical levels (i.e., clinician, region, province), but also the set-up of the data in terms of conducting statistical analyses. The organization of the database should only permit data to be entered in one format. Furthermore, considerations should be made for ensuring individual/clinician confidentiality. Finally, although mounting the database on the internet had accessibility appeal, many clinicians became frustrated with the speed of the dial up internet system that resulted in slow data entry. Providing Training and Ongoing Professional Development. It is important that all users receive consistent training on using the measure. For the Manitoba project, this training initially included sessions on how to access the database, enter individual information, and print reports. Subsequent sessions outlined shortcuts for data entry, reviewed methods for understanding and presenting results to employers, and identified methods to interpret results. The development of a provincial speech-language pathology outcomes measure has been an exciting challenge for many stakeholders who have sought to improve service delivery. It is anticipated that the measure will be developed further as formalized studies are conducted on the reliability, validity, and sensitivity of detecting change associated with the measure. Some of these studies are already in progress and the results are encouraging (Skwarchuk, Robertson, & Devlin, 2004; C. Johnson, personal communication, 2004). Furthermore, although the project has been locally endorsed, future work is needed to determine the broad applicability of the pediatric measure to other populations (e.g., adults), other regions in Canada and other parts of the world. National endorsement, followed by national statistics on prevalence rates and treatment effects, will contribute to the ultimate success of this project. Future studies could describe the prevalence and severity rates of the functional communication measures on a longitudinal basis as a function of many demographic variables. Knowledge about the composition of a very large caseload from across the province will aid in providing appropriate treatment options and ensure the overall viability of the profession. Acknowledgements The authors gratefully acknowledge the speech-language pathologists from the Interlake, South Central, and South East Regions who donated their time and efforts to the development of this project. Further thanks are extended to Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 Speech-Language Pathology Outcomes Measures all other S-LPs in Manitoba who have included their caseloads in the database and who have provided their commentaries in terms of further developing the measure. Thanks also to Manitoba Education, Citizenship and Youth consultants and support staff for their assistance with the project, and to two anonymous reviewers who provided commentary on an earlier version of the manuscript. Author Note Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Sheri-Lynn Skwarchuk, Faculty of Education and Extended Learning, University of Winnipeg, 515 Portage Avenue, Winnipeg, Manitoba, R3B 2E9, or through electronic mail at [email protected]. Mark Robertson, Manager, School Support Unit, may be reached at Manitoba Education, Citizenship and Youth, W310 1970 Ness Avenue, Winnipeg, Manitoba, R3J OY9, or through electronic mail at [email protected]. References Manitoba Teachers’ Society (2003). Beginning teaching. Retrieved April 14, 2003 from www.mbteach.org/begin.teach.html. Proactive Information Services (1998). The Manitoba special education review: A future for special education in Manitoba. Winnipeg, MB: Proactive Information Services, Inc. Reeves, D. B. (2002). Holistic accountability: Serving students, schools and community. Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin Press. Schooling, T. (2000). NOMS bears fruit. ASHA Leader, 5, 4-6. Skwarchuk, S. L. (2004). Teacher attitudes toward mandated standardized testing. Alberta Educational Research Journal, 50, 252-282. Skwarchuk, S.L., Robertson, M., & Devlin, D. (2004, June). Introducing a speechlanguage pathology outcomes measure in Manitoba. Paper presented at the Canadian Society for Studies in Education Annual Conference, Winnipeg, Manitoba. Swigert, N. B. (1997). National treatment outcomes data for professionals and consumers. ASHA Leader, 39, 49-51. Weber, K. J. (1994). Special education in Canadian schools. Thornhill, ON: Highland Press. Western Canadian Protocol (1996). Common curriculum framework for K-12 mathematics: Grade 10 to grade 12: The western Canadian protocol for collaboration in basic education. Winnipeg, MB: Manitoba Education and Training. Western Canadian Protocol (1998). Common curriculum framework for English language arts, kindergarten to grade 12: The western Canadian protocol for collaboration in basic education. Winnipeg, MB: Manitoba Education and Training. Received: July 4, 2003 Alberta Health (1993). Structure, process and outcome standards for Alberta health unit speech-language pathology programs. Edmonton, AB: Alberta Health. American Speech-Language Hearing Association (1996). User’s guide Phase I Group III: National treatment outcome data collection project. Rockville, MD: American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. American Speech-Language Hearing Association (1997). User’s guide: K-12 education: National center for treatment effectiveness in communication disorders, outcomes data collection project. Rockville, MD: American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. Cozby, P. C. (1993). Methods in behavioral research (5th Edition). Toronto, ON: Mayfield Publishing Company. Enderby, P. M. (1997). Therapy outcome measure (TOM): Speech language pathology. San Diego, CA: Singular Group, Inc. Eval-Plan Consulting (1998). Priority rating scale focus group report. New Brunswick: Eval-Plan Consulting. Gallagher, T. M., Swigert, N. B. & Baum, H. M. (1998). Collecting outcomes data in schools: Needs and challenges. Language, Speech, and Hearing in Schools, 29 , 250-256. Gay, L. R. & Airasian, P. (1996). Educational research: Competencies for analysis and application (6th Ed). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Merrill. Goldberg, B. (1997). ASHA NOMS: A case study. American Speech and Hearing Association, 39, 36-38. Gronlund, N. E. & Cameron, I. J. (2003). Assessment of student achievement. Toronto, ON: Pearson. John, A. & Enderby, P. (2000). Reliability of speech and language therapists using therapy outcome measures. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 35, 287-302. Johnson, B. & Christensen, L. (2000). Educational research: Quantitative and qualitative approaches (pp. 207-215). Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Law, J., Lindsay, G., Peacey, N., Gascoigne, M., Soloff, N., Radford, J. & Band, S. (2001). Facilitating communication between education and health services: The provision for children with speech and language needs. British Journal of Special Education, 28, 133-137. Manitoba Education and Training (1996). Success for all learners: A handbook on differentiating instruction. Winnipeg, MB: Manitoba Education and Training. Manitoba Education and Training (1997a). Grades 5 to 8 Mathematics: A foundation for implementation. Winnipeg, MB: Manitoba Education and Training. Manitoba Education and Training (1997b). Reporting on student progress and achievement. Winnipeg: MB: Manitoba Education and Training. Manitoba Education and Training (1998). Kindergarten to grade 4 English language arts: A foundation for implementation (part 1) overview; Learning outcomes K-4; Kindergarten, grades 1-2. Winnipeg, MB: Manitoba Education and Training. Manitoba Education and Training (1999). Policies and procedures for provincial examinations and standards tests. Winnipeg, MB: Manitoba Education and Training. Manitoba Education and Training (2000). Manitoba speech-language pathology outcomes measure: An implementation manual for speech-language pathologists. Winnipeg, MB: Manitoba Education and Training. Manitoba Education and Youth (2003a). Manitoba speech-language pathology outcomes measure. Retrieved April 22, 2003 from http://www.edu.gov.mb.ca/ks4/ specedu/slp/index.html. Manitoba Education and Youth (2003b). Manitoba speech-language pathology outcomes measure: 2001-2002 provincial report. Winnipeg, MB: Manitoba, Education and Youth. Accepted: July 28, 2005 Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 25 Predicting Reading from Oral Language Predicting Reading Abilities from Oral Language Skills: A Critical Review of the Literature Prévoir les aptitudes à la lecture à partir des capacités langagières : un compte rendu documentaire Elizabeth Ekins Phyllis Schneider Abstract The early identification of children who may be at risk for reading difficulty is important so that intervention can be provided early and subsequent reading problems can be avoided. Traditionally, children with reading problems are identified after reading instruction has begun. However, knowledge of oral language skills that predict reading abilities is necessary to identify children who may be at risk for later reading problems. This document discusses research literature documenting oral language measures that may predict lower level (decoding) and higher level (comprehension) reading abilities as well as reader-group membership. The predictive ability of expressive language, receptive language, Rapid Automatized Naming (RAN), and phonological awareness are discussed. The research results indicate that some oral language measures predict reading achievement. Although standardized measures of expressive language account for some variance in lower level reading, phonological awareness and RAN account for more. Standardized measures of expressive and receptive language predict reading comprehension in Grade 2. RAN tasks predict lower level reading and reader-group membership. Various combinations of syllable and phoneme deletion, syllable and phoneme blending, and rhyme detection predict lower level reading achievement, reading comprehension, or reader-group membership. The research information reviewed here can help guide future investigations in the area of predicting reading abilities. Abrégé Il est important de repérer très tôt les enfants qui risquent d’avoir de la difficulté à lire, afin de pouvoir intervenir rapidement et prévenir l’apparition de troubles ultérieurs. Habituellement, nous arrivons à identifier les enfants éprouvant de la difficulté à lire après le début de l’enseignement de la lecture. Toutefois, il est nécessaire de connaître au préalable les capacités langagières qui permettent de prédire les aptitudes à la lecture. Elizabeth Ekins, M.S.L.P., S-LP(C) Royal Inland Hospital Kamloops, BC Canada Phyllis Schneider, PhD Department of Speech Pathology and Audiology University of Alberta Edmonton, AB Canada Le présent article traite des recherches sur les mesures de la capacité langagière qui peuvent prédire les aptitudes à la lecture de niveau inférieur (décodage) et de niveau supérieur (compréhension) de même que l’adhésion à un groupe de lecture. Il examine la valeur prédictive de l’expression orale du langage, du langage dans son versant réceptif, de la dénomination rapide automatisée (DRA) et de la reconnaissance des sons. Les résultats de la recherche indiquent que certaines mesures du langage verbal permettent de prédire le niveau de lecture. Bien que les mesures normalisées de l’expression orale du langage expliquent certaines variations des capacités de lecture au niveau inférieur, la reconnaissance des sons et la DRA en expliquent davantage. Les mesures normalisées de l’expression orale du langage et du langage dans son versant réceptif permettent de prédire la compréhension en deuxième année. Les exercices de la DRA permettent de prédire les capacités de lecture au niveau inférieur et l’adhésion éventuelle à un groupe de lecture. Il est possible de prédire la capacité de lecture au niveau inférieur, la compréhension de ce que l’enfant lira ou l’adhésion éventuelle à un groupe de lecture selon que l’enfant combine la suppression de syllabes et de phonèmes, la confusion de syllabes et de phonèmes et la détection des rimes. Les résultats de la recherche dont on donne ici le compte rendu peuvent aider à orienter des études futures dans le domaine de la prédiction des aptitudes à la lecture. Ke y Words: reading, oral language, receptive language, expressive language, phonological awareness 26 Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 Predicting Reading From Oral Language S peech-language pathologists (S-LPs) have been viewed as experts on speech and language, yet have not had extensive involvement in children’s development of reading skills. However, given the growing view that reading is a language-based skill combined with S-LPs’ expertise in the area of language, S-LPs are becoming more actively involved in the assessment of and the intervention for reading disabilities. Further, S-LPs potentially have an important role in the process because oral language problems develop before children receive formal reading instruction. Since S-LPs often see these children first in the preschool years, they can play an important role in the early identification of reading problems. Traditionally, reading disabilities have been identified after reading instruction has begun (Catts, 1997). That is, children with reading problems are identified when they receive reading instruction in school and experience significant difficulties in learning to read. However, the number of children with early language disorders who eventually experience reading problems is estimated to be around 50% (Catts & Kamhi, 1999). Oral language does not need to be exceedingly weak to be related to poor reading; children may be found to be at risk even though present oral language status does not qualify them for S-LP services (Catts, Fey, Zhang, & Tomblin, 2001). Some negative consequences of reading problems include decreased motivation (Taylor, Harris, Pearson, & Garcia, 1995), lowered expectations of one’s abilities, and falling further behind peers in reading and consequently in academic achievement (Spear-Swerling & Sternberg, 1994). These negative consequences provide support for the early identification of children at risk for reading difficulties to prevent subsequent reading problems. Once these children are identified, treatment could begin before reading instruction. Such intervention may decrease any negative effects of reading failure on a child’s self-esteem, encourage academic achievement, and foster a positive outlook on reading and the value of reading abilities. An SLP’s expertise on language should lead to his/her involvement in program design to help decrease a child’s risk for future reading problems. Oral language is a broad construct and for intervention to proceed, S-LPs need to know which of its many variables are the strongest predictors of reading ability. This paper summarizes and discusses the results of 13 research articles that examined oral language measures that may predict both lower level and higher level (comprehension) reading abilities. Studies included in the review were longitudinal studies conducted between 1990 and 2001 that used correlational or regression designs and included at least one measure of oral language as a predictor. After a short review of current views on oral language and reading, the research findings are summarized followed by a discussion of clinical implications. Statistical Methods of Identifying Predictors One method of identifying which measures predict later reading achievement is to look for correlations between oral language measures and reading tasks. Correlational research is used to analyze relationships between and among two or more variables. The strength and the direction (positive or negative) of the relationship are examined. The results of correlational research indicate an association or relationship. Causation cannot be attributed between the variables. Correlational research can be applied to both longitudinal and cross-sectional study designs. Longitudinal studies are more appropriate for predicting reading abilities because the same children are followed over a period of time and the relations among variables are examined across time. Studies using correlational techniques have established that a relationship exists between oral language abilities and reading achievement. However, correlational techniques do not automatically correct for multiple comparisons; they cannot reveal which of a set of variables is the best predictor of a criterion variable. Investigators can apply the statistical method of multiple regression to a longitudinal study to examine the contribution of different variables in predicting reading achievement. Multiple regression enables the identification of more than one predictor variable of a criterion variable and the relative predictive value of each predictor (Norman & Streiner, 1998; Rosenberg & Daly, 1993; Schiavetti & Metz, 2002). Hierarchical (or fixed-order) regression is a type of regression analysis. The predictor variables are entered into the equation in an order determined by the researcher (Tabachnick & Fidell, 1996). Stepwise multiple regression is another type of regression analysis. In this case, the order of the entry of variables is determined by correlations among the variables, with the independent variable most highly correlated with the dependent variable entered first. Measurement of Oral Language and Reading Ability A variety of standardized and nonstandardized measures of oral language have been used to predict reading ability. These measures can be categorized into phonological awareness, Rapid Automatized Naming (RAN), expressive language, and receptive language. A description of the measures used in all the studies reviewed can be found in Appendix A. Two approaches to reading have been discussed in the literature — lower level and higher level (Catts and Kamhi, 1999). Lower level reading abilities include sound-symbol correspondence and word recognition. When one reads written words, the word’s meaning can be accessed by two methods. The first is an indirect method of phonological representation in which the reader uses knowledge of phoneme-letter correspondence to recode the letters into their corresponding phonemes. The second method is a direct method by way of visual representation. A match is made between the visual configuration and a visual representation that is part of the mental lexicon for the particular word. The first method could be thought of as sounding a word out or decoding and the second method a whole-word approach. To assess lower level reading abilities, children say real words and nonwords, also referred to as pseudowords. Tasks involving real words are referred to as reading accuracy, real Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 27 Predicting Reading from Oral Language word tasks, or word identification (Bishop & Adams, 1990; Felton & Brown, 1990). Tasks involving nonwords are referred to as nonword or word attack tasks (Bishop & Adams, 1990; Felton & Brown, 1990; Muter & Snowling, 1998). Higher level reading abilities, or reading comprehension, enable one to understand sentences and paragraphs (Catts & Kamhi, 1999). Lower level reading skills may be the focus of attention when a child is learning to read, whereas comprehension skills may be a concern for older children who have mastered lower-level skills but now must read to learn. To assess higher level reading, children are typically instructed to answer questions based on a reading passage. A variety of standardized and nonstandardized tests assess lower level and higher level reading abilities. A list of the tests used in the studies reviewed can be found in Appendix A. Studies have tried to predict lower level and/or higher level reading skills from oral language measures (Badian, 2001; Bishop & Adams, 1990; Catts, 1993; Catts et al., 1999 & 2001; Felton & Brown, 1990; Hurford, Schauf, Bunce, Blaich, & Moore, 1994; Manis, Seidenberg, & Doi, 1999; McCormick, Stoner, & Duncan, 1994; Menyuk, Chesnick, Liebergott, Korngold, D’Agostino, & Belanger, 1991; Muter & Snowling, 1998; O’Connor & Jenkins, 1999; Snow, Tabors, Nicholson, & Kurland, 1995). Some studies have included lower level reading skills or higher level reading skills (Felton & Brown, 1990; Manis, Seidenberg, & Doi, 1999; Muter & Snowling; 1998), have included both lower level and higher level reading skills (Badian, 2001; Bishop & Adams, 1990; Catts, 1993; Catts et al., 1999; Menyuk et al., 1991), or have combined these two skills under one label called reading (McCormick et al., 1994; Snow, Tabors, Nicolson, & Kurland, 1995). It should be noted that when two skills are combined into one measure, it is impossible to determine which oral language skills predict lower level reading and which oral language skills predict higher level reading. Methods Criteria were outlined to determine the inclusion of studies in the literature review. Studies since 1990 were chosen because they seem to have a richer view on predicting reading skills and include phonological skills, narratives and standard tests in the studies. Studies prior to 1990 are generally limited to phonological skills and reading. Further, studies that used longitudinal designs were included in this review. These studies are more effective and appropriate for determining what oral language abilities predict later reading difficulties. Once the criteria were determined, an exhaustive search was completed using the online data bases ERIC, PsycInfo, and PUBMED as well as through secondary references. Summary of Studies Reviewed A brief summary of the participants as well as the oral language and reading measures used in each of the studies can be found in Appendix B. Although some studies included measures other than oral language and reading, only oral language and reading variables are of interest in this review. This section comments on general strengths and weaknesses of the studies. 28 It is important to comment on the number of participants in each study compared to the number of predictor variables entered in the regression analysis. When discussing regression analysis, Norman and Streiner (1998) suggest that the number of participants be 5 or 10 times the number of variables entered in the regression analysis. When the authors discuss logistic regression analysis and analysis of covariance, they suggest that the number of participants be 10 times the number of predictor variables. The authors recommend that caution be used when overinterpreting regression models based on relatively small samples. However, they do not define a small sample size. Other authors recommend that the sample size be 30 (Pedhazur, 1983) or 50 (Glass & Hopkins, 1996) subjects for every predictor variable, providing support for more than 5 subjects for every predictor variable. It seems that larger numbers of subjects for every predictor variable is more desirable. We will use the smallest recommendation of 5 subjects per variable as the minimum when evaluating studies. Some studies had an adequate number of participants based on Norman and Streiner’s (1998) suggestion of 10 participants for each predictor variable entered in the regression analysis (Badian, 2001; Bishop and Adams, 1990; Catts, 1993; Catts et al., 1999; Felton & Brown, 1990; Manis et al., 1999; ). Muter and Snowling (1998) were close to the suggestion of 10 subjects for every predictor variable. Menyuk et al. (1991) met the minimum of 5 participants for every predictor variable and McCormick et al. (1994) fell short of the minimum recommendation. Tabachnick and Fidell (1996) suggest that when predicting group membership and using a discriminant analysis or stepwise logistic regression, the smallest group size needs to exceed the number of predictor variables. All studies that predicted group membership (Catts et al., 2001; Hurford et al., 1994; Muter & Snowling, 1998; O’Connor & Jenkins, 1999; ) included an adequate number of participants. In one study, (Menyuk et al., 1991) the authors did not differentiate between kindergarten, Grade 1, and Grade 2 reading skills. Rather, these three levels were considered together. When all three grades are grouped together, it is possible to lose the accuracy in predicting because the predictability of skills may change with different ages. As well, it is possible that kindergarten children may not yet have learned decoding skills. Two studies (McCormick et al., 1994; Snow et al., 1995) combined lower level and higher level reading measures. It is difficult to interpret the findings of McCormick et al. (1994) because the reading measure, Iowa Tests of Basic Skills (Hieronymus, Hoover, & Lindquist, 1986), included both lower level and higher level skills. As well, Snow et al. (1995) combined the subtests of the Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT) (Jastak & Jastak, 1976). When the two skills are combined, one cannot determine which oral language skills predict lower level reading and which oral language skills predict higher level reading. Catts (1993) and Catts et al. (1999) entered scores from different tests into the regression analysis, thereby losing some information on the predictive value of individual tests. Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 Predicting Reading From Oral Language In one study (Catts, 1993), the oral language measures were entered into the regression analysis as receptive language, expressive language, RAN, and phonological awareness. In another study (Catts et al., 1999), the language measures were entered into the regression analysis as oral language, RAN, and phonological awareness. Most of the studies used some measure of intellectual ability to control for this factor. Some studies used a general measure of IQ (e.g., Catts, 1999; Felton & Brown, 1990; Muter & Snowling, 1999), while others used a measure of verbal IQ (Badian, 2001) or nonverbal IQ (Catts, 1993; Catts et al., 2001). Authors do not discuss why they chose one of these types over the other. However, there are important implications of each of these choices. If we are interested in the predictive ability of language measures on reading scores, then using either full-scale or verbal IQ is likely to remove some of the effect of the language measures, since the IQ tests will be testing at least some of the same aspects of language, and thus there is likely to be a smaller relationship between the language and reading measures. Studies that control for nonverbal IQ would have avoided this confound by controlling for non-verbal cognitive abilities only. Bishop and Adams’s (1990) study was the only one to look at regression results with the effects of verbal IQ as well as both verbal and nonverbal IQ, but they do not discuss the implications of these different analyses. In some studies, the authors do not indicate whether or not the potentially confounding variable of nonverbal ability was controlled for (Badian, 2001; Manis et al., 1999; McCormick, 1994). These factors are important to control for as differences may arise from them. For example, without controlling for the non-verbal abilities of children, one does not know if these abilities contribute to the prediction of reading skills. Authors chose to control for non-verbal abilities in a variety of ways. Catts (1993) is the only researcher who controlled for grade differences as a result of some students repeating a grade or being placed in an alternative classroom. A ‘matriculation’ variable was entered first into the regression analysis. It is possible that previous classroom exposure may influence reading ability. Many of the studies performed correlations on the variables before entering oral language measures into a regression analysis. Menyuk et al. (1991) did not perform correlations before entering predictor variables into regression analyses. Further, all studies, with the exception of Felton and Brown (1990), entered all the predictor variables into a regression analysis. Felton and Brown entered only the variables that showed the highest correlations to reading outcome and that were representative of the three areas of phonological processing in the study. According to Stevens (2002), entering only those variables that are the most correlated to the dependent variables tends to make the results sample specific and therefore unreplicable. Reader-Group Membership Some studies were interested in predicting reader-group membership rather than or in addition to predicting actual reading scores. This approach determines whether and to what degree the measures used in a particular study discriminated between previously identified groups of children and thus adds another dimension to the investigation of prediction. An individual measure may predict a reading measure in isolation, but it might not predict who would actually be identified as having a reading problem, which is likely to be based on problems in several skill areas. There have been a variety of methods used to classify reader-group membership. Menyuk et al. (1991) used a cutoff score of 79 on the WRAT to define poor readers. The authors state that this score indicates borderline or deficient reading abilities at the beginning stages. In Hurford et al. (1994), the three reading groups (nondisabled, reading disabled, and “garden variety” poor readers) were created according to the reading tasks and the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test – Revised (PPVT-R) performance at the end of grade 2. The authors state that PPVT-R scores were used as a measure of intellectual ability. The nondisabled group consisted of children whose standard scores were above 1 standard deviation below the mean on composite reading score. The children classified as reading disabled had standard scores equal to or below 1 standard deviation below the mean in composite reading, with PPVTR standard scores above 1 standard deviation below the mean. The garden-variety group had standard scores equal to or below 1 standard deviation below the mean in composite reading, but also had PPVT-R standard scores equal to or below 1 standard deviation below the mean. Muter et al. (1998) defined good readers as those having reading accuracy scores above the 75th percentile on the Neale Analysis of Reading Ability Test (Neale, 1966). Poor readers had reading accuracy scores below the 25th percentile on the same test. Badian (2001) defined poor readers as those children who scored below the 25th percentile on word recognition. Although not stated, this definition leaves good readers to be defined as those who scored above the 25th percentile on the word recognition task. O’Connor and Jenkins (1999) used the Word Identification and Word Attack subtests of the Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests- Revised (WRMT-R) (Woodcock, 1987) to classify the children into one of two groups (average readers and reading disabled). The authors do not describe in detail the criteria for classifying the children. Catts et al. (2001) defined reading difficulties as scores greater than 1 standard deviation below the mean on the composite measure of reading comprehension. This definition is consistent with Hurford et al. (1994). The authors felt that this definition is a compromise when compared to more liberal definitions and more conservative definitions of reading disabilities used in the literature. Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 29 Predicting Reading from Oral Language Thus, studies used different definitions of poor or disabled readers. Some were based on lower-level reading skills, others on higher-level skills, and others on composite reading scores. Cut-off scores ranged from at or below one standard deviation to the 25th percentile, which is well within the normal range of scores. Results This section summarizes those measures found to predict lower level reading, higher level comprehension skills, and reader-group membership. The predictive ability of phonological awareness, RAN, expressive language, and receptive language is discussed. Phonological Awareness Phonological awareness is the ability to attend to, reflect on or manipulate the speech sounds in a word. Five phonological awareness activities were explored in seven of the articles reviewed. The results indicate that some phonological measures predict reading achievement while other measures need further research before making firm conclusions. Syllable and phoneme deletion and blending are phonological awareness tasks that predict lower level (Catts, 1993; Catts et al., 1999; Manis et al., 1999; Muter & Snowling, 1998) and higher level (Catts, 1993; Manis et al., 1999) reading ability. In some studies, syllable and phoneme deletion combined were found to predict both lower level and higher level reading (Catts et al., 1999; Manis et al, 1998). Muter and Snowling (1998) found that phoneme deletion alone predicts lower level reading. If phoneme deletion alone can predict lower level reading, then fewer tests would need to be administered. One cannot determine how much syllable deletion contributes to the prediction of lower level reading or comprehension as this task was not considered independently from phoneme deletion in the studies reviewed. Catts (1993) found syllable and phoneme deletion, combined with syllable and phoneme blending, to predict both word identification and word attack in Grade 1 and Grade 2, accounting for 4% to 37% of the variance. Further, this measure accounted for 25% of the variance when predicting comprehension in Grade 2. Although phonological awareness tasks predicted comprehension in Grade 2, receptive and expressive language measures accounted for more variance when entered first in the regression analysis (Catts, 1993). None of the studies reviewed for this paper looked at syllable and phoneme blending independently of deletion tasks. Therefore, it is difficult to determine which task, deletion or blending, contributes more to predicting or if both tasks contribute equally. The research to date indicates that 1) syllable and phoneme deletion together predict lower level reading and comprehension, 2) phoneme deletion predicts lower level reading, 3) syllable and phoneme deletion and syllable and phoneme blending combined predict lower level reading. 30 A number of phonological measures require further research before one can conclude that these tasks predict reading. Syllable and phoneme segmenting were found to predict lower level and higher level reading in one study (Menyuk et al. 1991). The results of this study need to be interpreted with caution. First, the scores from kindergarten, Grade 1, and Grade 2 participants were grouped together. The relationships may not be the same at all these ages since reading level changes dramatically during this period. Second, the authors did not indicate whether they had controlled for potentially confounding variables such as nonverbal ability and children repeating grades. Finally, the inclusion of more than 130 participants would have strengthened the study given that 17 variables were in the regression analysis. Further, although syllable segmenting was found to predict lower level reading in Badian (2001), this task only accounted for a small amount of variance (never more than 7%) (Badian, 2001). Another measure, rhyme detection, was not found to predict lower level reading in one study (Muter & Snowling, 1998). However, the results from the study conducted by Badian (2001) indicated that rhyme detection predicted word reading in Grade 1, although it accounted for a small amount of variance (never more than 6%). Further, rhyme detection inconsistently predicted a small amount of variance in reading comprehension in this study. McCormick et al. (1994) found the identification of consonants at the beginning of words presented orally to significantly predict a combined reading score of lower level reading and reading comprehension. Consonant identification accounted for 34% of the variance. It is unclear which level of reading this measure predicts as lower level and higher level reading were combined as a composite score. Further, the amount of variance consonant identification accounts for is unclear because the amount of variance changed from 34% to 6% depending upon the order entered into the regression. The unclear results make it difficult to determine the predictive power of consonant identification, and thus additional research is needed. Syllable and phoneme segmenting, syllable and phoneme deletion, and rhyme detection were used in two studies to predict reader group membership (good versus poor reader), either on its own or with a combination of other tasks. Rhyme detection did not contribute to the prediction of group membership in the study conducted by Muter and Snowling (1998), but it did identify 71% of the poor readers and 85% of the good readers in the study conducted by Badian (2001). Differences between these two studies may be explained by the small number of participants, 34, used by Muter and Snowling. Badian’s study had a larger number of participants and thus had greater power to yield significant results. Another reason for differences may be explained by the definition of reader-group membership. Muter and Snowling had used composite reading scores to define poor readers, while Badian used word recognition only; it is possible that rhyme detection would be more closely related to word Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 Predicting Reading From Oral Language recognition, a lower-level reading skill, than to a composite reading score. Phoneme and syllable deletion seem to be predictors of reader-group membership when measured at an early age. Catts et al. (2001) determined phoneme and syllable deletion to be among the best five predictors of reader-group membership when measured in kindergarten. Muter and Snowling (1998) found phoneme deletion combined with nonword repetition measures to predict group membership when measured at age 5 or 6. Finally, O’Connor and Jenkins (1999) found that syllable deletion when combined with two other measures predicted group membership only when it was measured in early kindergarten. Of the two segmenting tasks, syllable and phoneme, phoneme segmenting predicts reader-group membership in kindergarten and in Grade 1 (O’Connor and Jenkins, 1999). Badian (2001) found syllable segmenting to classify most good readers, but only a small percentage of poor readers. Ideally, one would want the measurements used to classify correctly most of the members of both groups. Based on these results, syllable segmenting may not be a strong variable in predicting reader-group membership. Rapid Automatized Naming In rapid automatized naming (RAN) tasks, children are shown an array of items to name as quickly as possible proceeding left to right, row by row. Before beginning the task, the children demonstrate their ability to name each item in isolation. Five different RAN measures, numbers, letters, objects, colours, and animals, have been used in several of the studies reviewed as either a composite score or as a single score to predict lower level reading. The results of the studies reviewed indicate strong support for the prediction of lower level reading from RAN measures (Catts, 1993; Catts et al., 1999; Felton & Brown, 1990; Manis et al., 1999; Menyuk et al., 1991). For example, Felton & Brown calculated simple correlations between RAN measures and word identification and word attack skills for children in first grade. Correlations ranged from .19 to .30 and were stronger for word identification than for word attack. All correlations were significant except for the correlation between colours and word attack and letters and word attack. Catts et al. (1993) calculated correlations between RAN measures and word identification and word attack in first and second grade, all of which are significant. Correlations ranged from .35 to .56. Catts and his colleagues (1999) only included the rapid naming of animals. The correlation between this measure and the combination of second grade reading comprehension and word recognition was 0.424. The above results indicate that a variety of RAN measures can be used to predict lower level reading. One study remains the exception. Menyuk and her colleagues (1991) found RAN letters, rather than tasks with colours, numbers, or objects, to be the only significant RAN task to predict word identification and a composite reading score consisting of word identification, word attack, and comprehension. The methodology of this study differs from the other studies. RAN tasks were administered prior to kindergarten and subjects from all 3 grades were included in the correlations without considering grade as a factor, which may affect generalizability of the results. Catts and his colleagues (1999) offered an argument for the use of one RAN measure in research. The investigators included only RAN animals in their study because previous work (Catts, 1993) had shown that many kindergarten children, particularly those with language impairment, could not consistently name letters or numbers. However, correlations obtained by Felton and Brown (1990) for numbers and letters summarized above do not appear to support this argument. Although it is evident that lower level reading can be predicted from RAN measures, more research is needed in this area to determine which RAN measure, if any, is best at predicting lower level reading. Such research could support these initial findings that, in fact, it does not matter which RAN measure is used. Although RAN tasks were found to predict reading comprehension in the four studies, the results indicate that RAN measures may not be the strongest predictor of reading comprehension. In Catts’ 1993 study, when a composite score of RAN was entered first in the multiple regression analysis, it accounted for 16% of the variance. However, phonological awareness, receptive language, and expressive language, when entered first, accounted for much more variance in reading comprehension (25%, 35%, and 33% respectively). When phonological awareness was entered first, RAN accounted for 7% of the variance, when receptive language was entered first, RAN accounted for 4% of the variance, and when expressive language was entered first, RAN was not significant. In another study, Manis et al. (1999) found that RAN letters accounted for 4% of the variance and RAN digits 3% of the variance once prior reading level was accounted for in the hierarchical regression analyses. Finally, in the study conducted by Catts and his colleagues (1999), once RAN was entered into the hierarchical regression, it only accounted for an additional 7% of the variance in one instance and 2% of the variance in another. Menyuk and her colleagues (1991) found RAN letters to consistently account for the largest amount of the variance, accounting for 22.52 % for the WRAT, 1.35% for the Gray Oral Reading Test (GORT) (Weiderholt & Bryant, 1986) and 19.45% using the Test of Reading Comprehension (TORC) (Brown, Hammill, & Weiderholt, 1978). It is difficult to determine the predictive nature of RAN in this study because of the methodology concerns discussed earlier and the fact that reading comprehension and lower level reading abilities were measured together in two of the tests. RAN has been used to differentiate between reading groups (Catts et al., 2001; Menyuk et al., 1991; O’Connor & Jenkins, 1999). Again, the studies differed on which RAN score was used. One study used animals (Catts et al., 2001), Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 31 Predicting Reading from Oral Language one used letters (O’Connor & Jenkins, 1999), and another used four separate scores — animals, objects, numbers, and colours (Menyuk et al., 1991). O’Connor and Jenkins found RAN letters to be one of the best three predictors of group membership. It consistently predicted group membership when administered in kindergarten and Grade 1. O’Connors & Jenkins started with a large number of predictors before narrowing to the best three. Catts and his colleagues (2001) started with a large number of predictors. However, the logistic regression analysis identified five significant variables that predicted the probability of reading difficulties in Grade 2. RAN (animals) was the fifth best predictor. The scores of the five predictors are entered into an equation devised by the authors to determine the probability of reading problems in Grade 2. With a cut off score of 0.30, the specificity level (accuracy at identifying normal readers as such) was high at 91.1%, the sensitivity level (accuracy at identifying problem readers as such) was moderate at 73.5% and the percentage of false negatives was 4.9%. In the study conducted by Menyuk and her colleagues (1991), the seven battery measures of which RAN colours, numbers, letters and objects were a part, identified only 21.7% of the poor readers as poor readers. The combination of 17 intake and battery measures identified only 45.7% of the poor readers as poor readers. Given the methodological concerns previously discussed regarding the study conducted by Menyuk and her colleagues, it can be argued that more weight should be given to the results of the other two studies. It appears reasonable to include RAN letters and animals as predictors of reader group membership. Expressive Language Expressive language has not been studied extensively as a predictor of reading ability, with only 5 out of the 13 studies reviewed including measures of this (Bishop & Adams, 1990; Catts, 1993; Catts et al., 1999; Menyuk et al., 1991; Snow et al., 1993). The expressive language measures found to predict lower level reading skills in these studies were mean length of utterance (MLU), a cloze task, formal definitions, superordinates, narrative production, and standardized measures of expressive language. The measures that predicted reading comprehension included formal definition, narrative ability, and standardized measures. MLU, measure of syntactic skill, was found to predict reading accuracy and nonword reading at age 8 when measures were taken at 4 ½ and 5 ½ years of age (Bishop & Adams, 1990). MLU accounted for 43% to 48% of the variance. Granted that because this study is the only one to include MLU, the strength of MLU in predicting lower level reading in this study warrants further investigation. If the findings of Bishop and Adams are replicated, it would warrant the use of this measure in practice. MLU is a measure of linguistic productivity that predicts syntactic development in young, typically-developing children. Thus it represents an aspect of language distinct from phonological awareness measures. One advantage of using MLU in practice is its ease of use. Further, this study measured MLU in preschool. The validation of MLU as a 32 predictor would enable clinicians to use this measure with confidence to determine if preschoolers were to encounter later reading problems. The earlier a child is recognized to have future reading difficulties, the earlier intervention can begin. A cloze task, designed to measure syntactic skills, was found to predict word identification and comprehension composite scores using the GORT, accounting for 31% of the variance. It was also found to predict lower level reading behaviors measured with the WRAT-R, accounting for 34% of the variance (Menyuk et al., 1991). The authors did not specify if other subtests of the WRAT-R were used. It is difficult to determine exactly which aspect of reading their cloze task predicts when the WRAT-R and the GORT are used. The results of this study need to be interpreted with caution. First, the scores from kindergarten, Grade 1, and Grade 2 participants were grouped together. It is possible that kindergarten children may not yet have learned decoding skills. Second, this study is the only one that used a cloze task. Other concerns discussed previously were sample size and no indication of controlling for potentially confounding variables. However, if the usefulness of the cloze task as a predictor is replicated in subsequent studies, clinical practice would benefit with the inclusion of a cloze task in assessment materials. Snow, Tabors, Nicholson, and Kurland (1995) investigated how print knowledge, metalinguistic awareness, and oral language skills relate to each other and how these skills relate to later literacy achievement, lower level reading ability, comprehension, and spelling abilities. The authors found that narrative production was strongly related to the WRAT-R score, but not to the GORT score; in both of these composite scores, lower and higher reading skills are combined. Two semantic language tasks, one requiring the child to provide a formal definition of an item and another requiring the child to supply a superordinate, were found to be correlated to the WRAT-R and the GORT (Snow et al., 1995). These results need to be interpreted carefully as the WRAT-R and the GORT measure a range of reading skills. Bishop and Adams (1990) used the Bus Story (Renfrew, 1995) among other variables to predict reading comprehension using the Neale Analysis of Reading Ability. Using multiple regression analysis, narrative production at age 4 ½ was found to predict reading comprehension at age 8, accounting for 57% of the variance. This finding suggests that narrative production may be useful for predicting higher level reading abilities, if confirmed with additional research. The authors describe the Bus Story measure to be a measure of the ability to express semantic relationships. However, the explanation for its predictiveness may lie in the fact that it is a narrative measure, and as such taps the ability to use a number of language skills (syntax, semantics, and pragmatics) to convey a story to a listener. Narratives have been described as a bridge between oral and written language, and knowledge of story schemas is believed to be important for reading comprehension (Westby, 2005). Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 Predicting Reading From Oral Language Catts (1993) found expressive language skills measured by the Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test (Gardner, 1979), the Structured Photographic Expressive Language Test-II (SPELT-II) (Werner & Kresheck, 1983), and the sentence imitation and grammatical closure subtests of the Test of Language Development (TOLD-2) (Newcomer & Hammill, 1988) to predict word attack and word identification abilities in Grade 1 (24% and 14% respectively) and in Grade 2 (17% and 15% respectively) when entered first in the multiple regression. However, phonological awareness skills and RAN tasks accounted for more variance for both grades when entered into the regression analysis before the expressive language measures. Catts et al. (1999) combined expressive and receptive language skills to predict word recognition abilities (word identification and word attack) in Grade 2. These results are consistent with Catts’ 1993 study, but phonological awareness and expressive and receptive language accounted for a large and similar amount of variance when entered first in the 1999 study. Perhaps when expressive language is combined with receptive language skills to include a larger language skill base, they contributed as much to prediction of word recognition as phonological awareness. Although expressive language measures were found to predict lower level reading, other measures such as phonological awareness and RAN have been investigated much more often. The results predicting reading comprehension from expressive language abilities are inconsistent among the three studies that included this measure. It is important to remember that expressive language is a broad construct and was measured differently across studies. In one study (Menyuk et al., 1991), scores from expressive language tests did not predict reading comprehension when scores from kindergarten, Grade 1, and Grade 2 were combined. However, the cloze task did predict reading comprehension in this study. In two other studies, expressive language (Catts, 1993) and expressive language combined with other oral language measures (Catts et al., 1999) predicted Grade 2 reading comprehension (33% and 56% respectively). In the latter two studies, the language measures contributed as much or more to Grade 2 reading comprehension than the phonological awareness and RAN tasks. The study conducted by Menyuk and her colleagues may have different results because the comprehension scores were reported on one group of students from a variety of grades, most of whom probably had not developed enough reading comprehension for the relationship to have developed yet. At present, with the available research, expressive language measures collected in kindergarten appear to predict reading comprehension in Grade 2. Two studies included expressive language measures when predicting reader group membership (Catts et al., 2001; Menyuk et al., 1991). Menyuk and her colleagues included expressive language scores along with receptive language measures, an articulation measure, a verbal fluency measure, and an auditory discrimination measure. These measures correctly classified only 6.5% of the poor readers. When Menyuk and her colleagues combined all the predictor variables, they classified 46% of the poor readers. In the study conducted by Catts and his colleagues, expressive and receptive language skills were combined. This composite score was not a significant variable in predicting readergroup membership. The low classification rates in one study and the finding that expressive language scores are not significant predictors in another study suggest that expressive language scores are not an ideal predictor of reader-group membership, at least in the lower grades. Additional research is needed in the area of expressive language. Although a variety of expressive language skills have been studied, many have not been studied extensively. It would be beneficial to continue to use a range of expressive language subskills and analyze them separately and together. Further research is warranted in predicting reader-group membership since some expressive language skills have been shown to predict individual scores but not group membership. Receptive Language Like expressive language abilities, receptive language abilities have not been studied extensively in predicting reading achievement. Six of the studies reviewed included this independent variable (Bishop & Adams, 1990; Catts, 1993; Catts et al., 1999; McCormick, 1994; Menyuk et al., 1991; Snow et al., 1995). The results of the studies suggest that receptive language abilities are not the strongest predictors of lower level reading skills. Receptive vocabulary skills, measured using the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised (PPVTR) (Dunn & Dunn, 1981) or the British Picture Vocabulary Scale (BPVS) (Dunn, Dunn, Whetton, & Pintilie, 1982), either did not predict lower level skills (Bishop & Adams, 1990; Catts, 1993; McCormick et al., 1994; Menyuk, 1991) or accounted for little variance (10%) (Catts, 1993). PPVT-R was entered into the regression analysis with the Token Test for Children and the Test of Language Development – 2 (TOLD-2). There is one exception to this finding. Snow et al. (1995) found the PPVT-R to have a strong correlation with word identification (.44) and a strong correlation to scores on the Gray Oral Reading Test – Revised (GORT-R) (.48), a test that measures both lower level and comprehension abilities. It is difficult to compare this result to others because it is a bivariate correlation; studies in which the PPVT-R was used in multiple regressions may not have found it to be a predictor because the variance was better accounted for by other variables in the regression. The Token Test for Children, a measure of receptive language, was found to predict lower level reading (Catts, 1993; Menyuk et al., 1991). This test accounted for 25% of the variance in the study conducted by Menyuk and her colleagues. However, there were concerns regarding the methodology used by Menyuk and her colleagues as discussed earlier. Further, Catts entered the Token Test for Children into the multiple regression with the scores from the PPVT-R and TOLD-2; the results indicated that receptive language accounted for only a small amount of variance (10%) or was insignificant in some cases. Given these factors, Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 33 Predicting Reading from Oral Language the Token Test for Children cannot be considered a reliable predictor of lower level reading achievement. The TOLD-2 was used in two studies with inconsistent results. First, Catts (1993) entered the Grammatical Understanding subtest of the TOLD-2 into a multiple regression with the Token Test for Children and the PPVTR, and they accounted for no or little variance (10% to 17%). However, lower level reading was predicted when the TOLD2 was entered with the other receptive language scores as well as the expressive language scores (Catts et al., 1999). It is difficult to determine which area of language, receptive or expressive, is accounting for the prediction. It is possible that expressive language accounts for more of the prediction capabilities when receptive and expressive language scores are combined. Bishop and Adams (1990) found the Test for Reception of Grammar (TROG) (Bishop, 1989), a British test of receptive syntax, to predict reading accuracy, accounting for 52% of the variance at age 4 ½ and 51% at age 5 ½. Given the mixed results involving the TOLD-2 and the TROG, further investigation of receptive syntax would be beneficial. The above results indicate that syntactic measures of receptive language may be stronger predictors of lower level reading skills than semantic measures alone or as syntactic and semantic measures combined. Receptive language scores have been found to predict reading comprehension. Bishop and Adams (1990) determined that the TROG and the BPVS predicted reading comprehension at age 5 ½. The TROG accounted for 63% of the variance and the BPVS accounted for 57% of the variance. Catts (1993) determined that the PPVT-R, the Token Test for Children, and the TOLD-2, when entered together, predicted reading comprehension. These three tests accounted for 6% to 35% of the variance. Menyuk and her colleagues (1991) also determined that the Token Test for Children predicted comprehension, accounting for 10% of the variance. The Screening Test of Auditory Comprehension of Language (TACL) (Carrow, 1973) and the PPVT-R did not account for any variance, although the methodology concerns in Menyuk et al. make the results less reliable. Snow and her colleagues (1995) found PPVT-R scores to be strongly related to lower level and comprehension abilities measured by the GORT-R. However, in McCormick et al. (1994), PPVT-R failed to predict reading comprehension. There were two concerns with this study. First, the reading measure, the Iowa Test of Basic Skills, combines decoding and comprehension abilities as one score. When the two skills are combined, one cannot determine which oral language skills predict lower level reading and which oral language skills predict higher level reading. Second, the study may have lacked sufficient power given the number of variables entered into the regression analysis relative to the number of subjects. Given the results from Bishop and Adams (1990) and Catts (1993), receptive language tests that measure semantic and syntactic skills do appear to predict reading comprehension. Three studies included receptive language as possible predictors of reader-group membership. Two of these studies, Menyuk et al. (1990) and Catts et al., (2001), were discussed earlier. One study (Menyuk et al., 1990) found that 34 receptive language, when combined with the other measures used in the study, had low group prediction abilities. The other study (Catts et al., 2001) did not find receptive language scores, when combined with expressive language scores, to be a significant predictor of group membership. The third study (Hurford et al., 1994) included PPVT-R scores with other scores (phonological discrimination task and phonemic segmentation task) and had high group membership classification with only 2% being misclassified. However, the authors defined the reading disabled group as having weak reading abilities and typical receptive vocabulary abilities. This definition assumes that reading disabled children have normal language abilities. In contrast, many authors hold that most reading disabled children have accompanying language problems (Catts, 1997). As with expressive language scores, receptive language scores may not be ideal for predicting group membership. Given the limited scope of research in this area, more research is needed. In particular, it is important to consider expressive and receptive language skills separately in order to determine whether they each are predictive of reading skills or only predict when both are considered. Conclusions The research results from these studies indicate that some oral language measures predict lower level and higher level reading achievement as well as reader-group membership. Although standardized measures of expressive language account for some variance in lower level reading, other measures, phonological awareness and RAN, account for more. Standardized measures of expressive and receptive language are better than phonological awareness skills and RAN tasks when predicting reading comprehension in Grade 2. They seem to be more suited for predicting reading comprehension than for predicting lower level reading skills. RAN tasks predict lower level reading and reader-group membership. RAN also contributes to the prediction of reading comprehension, although it does not account for as much variance as other measures. Results of the studies indicate that syllable and phoneme deletion together, phoneme deletion alone, and syllable and phoneme deletion and blending all predict lower level reading achievement. Syllable and phoneme deletion together predict reading comprehension. Finally, rhyme detection and syllable and phoneme deletion predict reader-group membership. From the research results, one can start to create a battery of tests that predict reading. Measures that predict lower and higher level reading should be included in the battery as lower level reading skills are the foundation to reading and higher level reading enables one to understand sentences and paragraphs. Ideally the smallest number of tests should be used to maximize efficiency. Syllable and phoneme deletion and RAN could be included in a battery to predict lower level reading abilities. To predict higher level reading skills, the TOLD-2: P and a narration task, such as the Bus Story, could be included in the battery. Some measures require further research before making firm conclusions. More research is needed regarding MLU, Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 Predicting Reading From Oral Language cloze tasks, formal definition, and superordinates in relation to lower level reading. In addition, receptive language results are inconclusive for lower level reading because of inconsistencies among studies. Additional research is needed to determine the predictive nature of syllable and phoneme segmenting, rhyme detection, and consonant identification for reading comprehension. As well, more research is needed to determine whether or not syllable segmenting can be used to predict reader-group membership. When designing future research studies, there are some suggestions to keep in mind. First, the use of regression analysis enables the identification of the relative predictive value of a predictor variable. Second, it may be helpful to enter oral language measures independently into a regression analysis rather than grouping them into broad categories such as receptive language or expressive language. This would allow one to determine the specific measures that enable one to predict. Third, it is important to distinguish between lower level reading and higher level reading measures as a criterion variable. Fourth, it is important to include an adequate number of participants in the study. We recommend that the number of participants be at least 5 or 10 times the number of variables entered in the regression analysis. It is also important to control for nonverbal abilities when entering variables into the regression analysis to help ensure that the variance accounted for can be attributed to oral language skills and not nonverbal abilities. The conclusions made regarding the variables that predict reading achievement are based on those measures and tasks used in the studies reviewed here. Other measures such as memory, cognition, parent/child interactions, and mother’s education level may also be useful in predicting reading achievement. Clinical Application Research to date has provided insight on predicting reading problems from oral language skills and will help guide future research. S-LPs, who have an expertise in the area of language, will assess preschoolers’ language skills and thus can have an important role in helping to identify children who would be at risk for later reading problems in school. The research results suggest possible measures that can help determine whether or not a young child would be at risk for future reading problems. Phonological measures, such as syllable and phoneme deletion and syllable and phoneme blending, as well as RAN measures predict lower level reading skills. Further, standard measures of expressive and receptive language predict reading comprehension abilities. It is important to remember that a child’s score that is within normal limits on language tests does not guarantee that the child will not have future reading difficulties. Once these measures are collected, the information needs to be utilized to benefit the child. There are a few available options. First, the child could receive early intervention regarding language skills from an S-LP to help eliminate or reduce future reading difficulties. This may be challenging given the large caseloads of S-LPs. However, it need not involve additional therapy — the S-LP can choose to work on language goals that will benefit both oral and written language. As well, suggestions can be made to classroom teachers about activities from which all students can benefit. Specific phonological awareness activities can be incorporated into speech and language therapy. Rhyme awareness is a beginning form of phonological awareness because it involves an ability to analyze words at the level of the onset and rime. When treating phonological processes minimal pairs and nonsense words can be used. Children’s books written using rhymes can be utilized in therapy. The concept of segmentation can be incorporated into therapy by the careful selection of words. For example, compound words and their parts can be used to indirectly demonstrate syllable deletion (e.g. ‘cowboy’, ‘cow’, and ‘boy’). The work that S-LPs do to improve oral language skills contributes to children’s reading comprehension abilities. A second way to utilize the information to benefit the child is to inform the child’s parents or caregivers about how early oral language difficulties can impact later reading skills. With this information, parents or caregivers, with the support of an S-LP, may be able to implement a home program to benefit the child. As well, parents or caregivers can help to transfer the information to the school when the child enters kindergarten. Third, it is important to increase the awareness of S-LPs and other professionals, such as teachers, regarding the relationship of oral language skills and reading abilities. This will enable professionals who did not work with the child in preschool to apply the knowledge of oral language measures to the acquisition of reading in school. In addition, shared knowledge about this topic among those involved with the child will increase the child’s support system. The current research, combined with future investigations regarding the predictive nature of oral language skills, will assist in the early identification of children who are at risk for reading difficulties. References Badian, N.A. (2001). Phonological and orthographic processing: Their roles in reading prediction. Annals of Dyslexia, 51, 179-202. Beery, K.E. (1982). Revised administration, scoring and teaching manual for the Developmental Test of Visual Motor Integration. Cleveland, OH: Modern Curriculum Press. Bishop, D.V.M., & Adams, C. (1990). A prospective study of the relationship between specific language impairment, phonological disorders and reading retardation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry , 31, 1927-1050. Bishop, D.V.M. (1989). Test for Reception of Grammar (2nd ed.). Age and Cognitive Performance Research Centre, University of Manchester. Bradley, L. & Bryant, P.E. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read – a causal connection. Nature, 301, 419-421. Brown, U., Hammill, D., & Weiderholt, J. (1978). Test of Reading Comprehension: A method of assessing the understanding of written language. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. Carrow, E. (1973). Screening Test for Auditory Comprehension of Language. Boston, MA: Testing Resources Corporation. Catts, H.W. (1993). The relationship between speech-language impairments and reading disabilities. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 948-958. Catts, H.W. (1997). The early identification of language-based reading disabilities. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 28 , 86-89. Catts, H. W., Fey, M. E., Zhang, X., & Tomblin, J. B. (1999). Language basis of reading and reading disabilities: Evidence from a longitudinal investigation. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 331-361. Catts, H. W., Fey, M. E., Zhang, X., & Tomblin, J. B. (2001). Estimating the risk of future reading difficulties in kindergarten children: A research-based model and its clinical implications. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 32, 38-50. Catts, H. W., & Kamhi, A. G. (Eds.). (1999). Language and reading disabilities . Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon. Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 35 Predicting Reading from Oral Language DeRenzi, E. & Gerrari, C. (1978). The Reporters Test: A sensitive test to detect expressive disturbances in aphasia. Cortex, 14, 279-293. DiSimoni, F. (1978). The Token Test for Children. Boston, MA: Teaching Resources Corporation. Dunn, L.M., & Dunn, L.M., (1981). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test- Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. Dunn, L.M., Dunn, L.M., Whetton, C., & Pintilie, D. (1982). British Picture Vocabulary Scale. Windsor: NFER-Nelson. Elliott, C.D., Murray, D.J., & Pearson, L. (1978). British Ability Scales . Windsor: NFER-Nelson. Felton, R.H., & Brown, I.S. (1990). Phonological processes as predictors of specific reading skills in children at risk for reading failure. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2, 39-59. Glass, G.V., & Hopkins, K.D. (1996). Statistical methods in education and psychology. (3rd Ed.). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn and Bacon. Gardner, M.F. (1979). Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test. Novato, CA: Academic Therapy Publications. Garson, D. Regression Analysis. Retrieved March 27, 2005, from http:// www2.chass.ncsu.edu/garson/pa765/regress.htm. Goldman, R., & Fristoe, M. (1986). Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. Hieronymus, A. N., Hoover, H. D., & Lindquist, I. F. (1986). Iowa Test of Basic Skills. Chicago, IL: Riverside Publications. Hurford, D. P., Schauf, J.D., Bunce, L., Blaich, T., & Moore, K. (1994). Early identification of children at risk for reading disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27, 371-382. Jastak, S., & Jastak, S. (1976). Wide Range Achievement Test. Wilmington, DE: Guidance Associates. Kaplan, E., Goodglass, H. & Weintraub, S. (1982). Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia: Lea & Fibiger. Kirk, S., McCarthy, J., & Kirk, W. (1968). Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic Ability. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press. Lee, L. (1974). Developmental sentence scoring procedures. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press. Lindamood, C.H. & Lindamood, P.C. (1979). Lindamood Auditory Conceptualization Test. Boston: Teaching Resources Corporation. Manis, F.R., Seidenberg, M.S., & Doi, L.M. (1999). See Dick RAN: Rapid naming and the longitudinal prediction of reading subskills in first and second graders. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 129-157. Mason, J., & Stewart, J. (1989). Early Childhood Diagnostic Instrument. Iowa City, IA: American Testronics. McCarthy, D. (1970). McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities. New York: Psychological Corporation. McCormick, C.E., Stoner, S.B., & Duncan, S. (1994). Kindergarten predictors of first-grade reading achievement: A regular classroom sample. Psychological Reports, 74, 403-407. Menyuk. P., Chesnick, M., Liebergott, J.W., Korngold, B., D’Agostino, R., & Belanger, A. (1991). Predicting reading problems in at-risk children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 893-903. Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M., & Taylor, S. (1997). Segmentation, not rhyming, predicts early progress in learning to read. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 58, 112-133. Muter, V., & Snowling, M. (1998). Concurrent and longitudinal predictors of reading: The role of metalinguistic and short-term memory skills. Reading Research Quarterly, 33, 320-337. Muter, V., Snowling, M., & Taylor, S. (1994). Orthographic analogies and phonological awareness: Their role and significance in early reading development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 35, 293-310. Neale, M. D. (1966). Neale Analysis of Reading Ability. London: Macmillan. Newcomer, P. L. (1990). Diagnostic Achievement Battery -2. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. Newcomer, P. L., & Hammill, D. D. (1988). Test of Language Development-2 Primary. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. Norman, G. R., & Streiner, D. L. (1998). Biostatistics: The bare essentials. Hamilton: B.C. Decker Inc. Nurss, J. R., & McGauvran, M. E. (1976). Metropolitan Readiness Test. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. O’Connor, R. E., & Jenkins, J. R. (1999). Prediction of reading disabilities in kindergarten and first grade. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 159-197. Owens, R. E. (2001). Language development: An introduction. (5th ed.). Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Pedhazur, E. J. (1983). Multiple regression in behavioral research: Explanation and prediction. (2nd Ed.). New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Wilson. Psychological Corporation. (1992). Stanford Achievement Test (8th Ed.). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Brace. Renfrew, C. (1995). The Bus Story: A test of continuous speech. Old Headington, Oxford, UK: Renfrew. Rosenberg, K. M., & Daly, H. B. (1993). Foundations of behavioral research: A basic question approach. Fort Worth: Harcourt, Brace, & Jovanovich. Scarborough, H. S. (1998). Early identification of children at risk for reading disabilities: Phonological awareness and some other promising predictors. In B. K. Shapiro, P. J. Accardo, & A. J. Capute (Eds.), Specific reading disability: A view of the spectrum (pp. 75-119). Timonium, MD: York Press. Schiavetti, N., & Metz, D. E. (2002). Evaluating research in communicative disorders. Boston: Allyn and Bacon. 36 Semel, E., Wiig, E., & Secord, W. (1987). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals- Revised. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. Silveroli, N.J. (1984). Classroom Reading Inventory (4th ed.). Dubuque, IA: Brown. Snow, C.E., Tabors, P.O., Nicholson, P.A., & Kurland, B.F. (1995). SHELL: Oral language and early literacy skills in kindergarten and first-grade children. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 10 , 37-48. Spear-Swerling, L., & Sternberg, R. J. (1994). The road not taken: An integrative theoretical model of reading disability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27, 91-103. Stevens, S. (2002). Applied multivariate statistics for the social sciences (4 th Ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (1996). Using multivariate statistics (3rd Ed.). New York: Harpercollins College Publishers. Taylor, B., Harris, L. A., Pearson, P. D., & Garcia, G. (1995). Reading difficulties: Instruction and assessment. New York: McGraw-Hill, Inc. Templin, M., & Darley, F. (1969). Articulation Screening Test. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press. Werner, E. O., & Kresheck, J. D. (1983). Structured Photographic Expressive Language Test-II. Sandwich, IL: Janelle Publications. Wechsler, D. (1967). Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence. New York: Psychological Corporation. Westby, C. E. (2005). Assessing and remediating text comprehension problems. In H. W. Catts & A. G. Kamhi (Eds.), Language and reading disabilities (2nd Ed.), pp. 157-232. Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Wiederhold, J., & Bryant, B. (1986). Gray Oral Reading Test. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. Woodcock, R. W. (1987). Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests-Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. Acknowledgements We would like to extend our thanks to Bonnie Dobbs, professor and colleague, for assisting with statistical analysis, as well as the Editor at the time, Phil Doyle, and reviewers of JSLPA for guidance during the editorial process. Author note Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Elizabeth Ekins, Rehabilitation Services, Royal Inland Hospital, 311 Columbia Street, Kamloops, BC V2C 2T1, [email protected] Received: November 20, 2003 Accepted: April 25, 2005 Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 Predicting Reading From Oral Language APPENDIX A Oral Language and Reading Measures The standardized and nonstandardized tests are described as they are in the articles, with a description of either the task or the skills that the task measures. In some cases, such as with a rhyme task, authors used variations to a similar task. Phonological Aw areness-Standardiz ed Tests Lindamood Auditory Conceptualization Test (Lindamood & Lindamood, 1979) The child manipulates different coloured blocks to indicate conceptualization of the speech sound patterns presented by the examiner Phonological Aw areness-Nonstandardiz ed Tests Initial Consonant Not Same (Felton & Brown, 1990) Four words are spoken by the examiner and the child chooses the word that does not begin with the same sound as the first word in the list Final Consonant Different (Felton & Brown, 1990) Four words are spoken by examiner and child chooses the word that has a different ending sound from the other words Rhyme (Felton & Brown, 1990) The child names as many words as he can that rhyme with a given word Rhyme production (O'Connor & Jenkins, 1990) The child is required to provide a rhyme to a given word Rhyme detection (Muter & Snowling, 1998; Badian, 2001) Given the pictures of three words, the child had to indicate which of the three words, supplied by the examiner, rhymed with the target word. All words were depicted by a drawing, the examiner supplying the names of all four words in each item Syllable Counting Test (Felton & Brown, 1990) The examiner pronounces 1,2 or 3 syllable words and the child uses a wooden dowel to tap out the number of syllables heard Word String Memory Test (Felton & Brown, 1990) The child repeats a string of four words after the examiner. Four strings are composed of rhyming words and four strings are not. Researchers consider this phonetic recoding in working memory Deletion task -- syllable (Catts, 1993; Catts et al., 1999 & 2001; Manis et al., 1999; O'Connor & Jenkins, 1999) Requires the child to delete a syllable from a compound word or a two-syllable word and say the remaining sound sequence Deletion task -- initial phoneme (Muter & Snowling, 1998) Requires the child to delete the initial phoneme from a word and say the remaining sound sequence Deletion task -- phoneme (Catts, 1993; Catts et al., 1999 & 2001; Manis et al., 1999) Requires the child to delete a phoneme from a word and say the remaining sound sequence Blending task -- syllable (Catts, 1993; O'Connor & Jenkins, 1999) Requires the child to blend together and pronounce syllables Blending task -- phoneme (Catts, 1993; O'Connor & Jenkins, 1999) Requires the child to blend together and pronounce phonemes Segmenting -- syllable (Menyuk et al., 1991; Badian, 2001) Requires the child to say the word broken into syllables. Another version requires the child to tap the number of syllables in a given word Segmenting -- phonemes (Menyuk et al., 1991; O'Connor & Jenkins, 1999) Requires the child to segment monosyllabic words Consonant identification task (McCormick et al., 1994) The child names the letter for the beginning sound of a spoken word Phonological discrimination task (Hurford et al., 1994) Requires the child to compare a standard pair of syllables to a comparison pair separated by 1,000 ms. Each syllable within a pair is separated by intersyllable intervals of 10, 80 or 160 ms (e.g. /gi/10ms/gi/1,000ms/di/10ms/gi/) Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 37 Predicting Reading from Oral Language APPENDIX A Oral Language and Reading Measures (continued) Phonemic segmentation task (O'Connor & Jenkins, 1999) The child repeats a CVC word or pseudoword after the examiner and then pronounces the word again without the designated consonant (initial or final) Non-word repetition test (Muter & Snowling, 1998) The child repeats 40 nonwords of between one and four syllables in length Sound repetition (O'Connor & Jenkins, 1999) The child repeats isolated phonemes separated with a 0.5 second pause Rapid letter naming (O'Connor & Jenkins, 1999) The child is presented with a card of 60 randomly ordered letters in large uppercase type and is asked to name as many letters as he can in 1 minute First-sound isolation (O'Connor & Jenkins, 1999) Requires the child to say the first sound of an orally given word (i.e. "Tell me the first sound of _____.") Rapid Automatiz ed Reading (RAN) RAN requires the child to name representations of familiar items, such as common objects, colours, letters, numbers, or animals, presented in a series as rapidly as possible. This is a timed test and a lower score is more desirable. The items are displayed in an array and are named from left to right, row by row. The child demonstrates the ability to name each item in isolation before beginning. Expressive language- Standardiz ed Tests Bus Story Test- Information Score Standardized story retell test- the child retells a story while looking at a picture book; the amount of information recalled is totalled Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test Measures expressive vocabulary of single words Structured Photographic Expressive Language Test- II Declarative, interrogative, negative and embedded sentences are tested (SPELT-II) Test of Language Development-2:Primary (TOLD-2:P) Sentence Imitation, Grammatical Completion, and Oral Vocabulary (Newcomer & Hammill, 1988) subtests 38 Boston Naming Test (Kaplan, Goodglass, & Weintraub, 1982) Confrontation naming task; the child names line drawings Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic Ability (ITPA) (Kirk, McCarthy, & Kirk, 1968) grammatical closure subtest Screens the expression of syntactic class relations and morphological markers Development Sentence Scoring Procedures (DSS) (Lee, 1974) Measures syntactic structures in spontaneous speech Reporters Test (DeRenzi & Ferrari, 1978) Measures expression of semantic relations in utterances Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation (Goldman & Fristoe, 1986) Assesses the child's phonological skills Templin-Darley Articulation Screening Test (Templin & Darley, 1969) Assesses the child's phonological skills McCarthy Scales of Children's Abilities --verbal fluency subtest (McCarthy, 1970) The child is asked to name as many articles in a given category as possible within 20 seconds Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 Predicting Reading From Oral Language APPENDIX A Oral Language and Reading Measures (continued) Expressive Language- Nonstandardiz ed Tests Mean Length of Utterance (MLU) (Bishop & Adams, 1990) The number of morphemes are calculated in relation to the number of utterances spoken by the child to measure syntactic development Expressive phonology (Bishop & Adams, 1990) Measured by the percentage of consonants correct in a picture naming task Cloze (Menyuk et al, 1991) The child is required to provide a word that has been left out of a sentence Story recall (Menyuk et al., 1991) The child listens to a story over headphones and retells the story to a puppet Sementactic judgement (Menyuk et al., 1991) The child is required to determine if a sentence is grammatically correct. If it is not, the child needs to correct the sentence Word recall (Menyuk et al., 1991) The child learns the names of pictures and then is required to tell the items back to the examiner without the use of the pictures Narrative production (Snow et al., 1995) The child tells a story based on three slides Picture description (Snow et al., 1995) Requires the child to describe a picture as completely as possible Definitions (Snow et al., 1995) The child defines 14 familiar nouns (e.g. bird, alphabet) Narrative story task (Catts et al., 1999 & 2001) Assesses child's abilities to comprehend, organize, and retell the major details of a story read aloud by the examiner Receptive Language- Standardiz edTests British Picture Vocabulary Scale (Dunn, Dunn, Whetton, & Pintilie, 1982) A British version of the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test - Revised Child selects from an array of pictures that match the word spoken by examiner Test for Reception of Grammar (TROG) (Bishop, 1989) The child selects from an array of pictures that match the phrase or sentences spoken by examiner British Abilities Scales (BAS) (Elliot, Murray, & Pearson, 1978) Verbal comprehension subtest- a general measure of the ability to carry out instructions Naming vocabulary subtest - confrontation naming task Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised (PPVT-R) (Dunn & Dunn, 1981) The child selects from four pictures the one that best represents a word read by the examiner Token Test for Children (DiSimoni, 1978) Measures semantic comprehension in sentences Test of Language Development-2:Primary (TOLD-2) (Newcomer & Hammill, 1988) Grammatical understanding, picture vocabulary subtests auditory discrimination subtest Metropolitan Readiness Test-Level II, Form P (Nurss & McGauvran, 1976) Orally administered readiness test requiring the child to mark responses in a test booklet. Three components were 1) Auditorydiscrimination of initial sounds and sound-symbol association, 2) visual discrimination among visual symbols and separation of visual patterns from context, 3) language- cognitive concepts, grammatical structures of standard English, and listening skills Screening Test of Auditory Comprehension of Language (TACL) (Carrow, 1973) Screens comprehension of syntactic classes and relations and morphological classes and relations Clinical Evaluation of Language FundamentalsRevised (CELF-R) (Semel, Wiig, & Secord, 1987) Listening to Paragraphs subtest Receptive Language- Nonstandardiz ed Tests Story comprehension task (Snow et al., 1995) Comprehension questions are asked while a story is read aloud Comprehension of complex sentences (Menyuk et al., 1991) The child is asked a question about what happened in a complex sentence. Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 39 Predicting Reading from Oral Language APPENDIX A Oral Language and Reading Measures (continued) Low er Level Reading- Standardiz ed Tests Neale Analysis of Reading Ability, form C As the child reads stories a loud, errors in reading words are scored Woodcock Reading Mastery Test - form A (WRMT-R) Word Identification subtest- untimed reading of a list of sight words Word Attack subtest- untimed reading of a list of pseudo-words Gray Oral Reading Test - Revised (GORT-R) (Wiederhold, & Bryant, 1986) Measures ability to read passages with speed and accuracy, assesses oral reading comprehension, and provides a total measure of reading performance Iowa Tests of Basic Skills, Early Primary Battery, Level 6 (Hieronymus, Hoover, & Lindquist, 1986) Measures the child's ability to read words in isolation and to use context and picture cues for word identification. The children were also asked sentence and story comprehension questions. Wide Range Achievement Test- Revised (WRAT-R) (Jastak & Jastak, 1976) Sight word recognition subtest Graded Nonword Reading Test (Muter & Snowling, 1998) The child is required to read 20 nonwords, 10 one-syllable words and 10 two-syllable words Stanford Achievement Test, 8th Edition (SAT) (Psychological Corporation, 1992) Word reading subtest- The child reads several words and decides which word tells about a picture Word study skills subtest- Measures phonological awareness and knowledge of grapheme-phoneme relationships Low er Level Reading - Nonstandardiz ed Test Reading of non-words (Bishop & Adams, 1990) The child reads a list of non-words (e.g. bab, wob, zok) Exception-word reading task (Manis et al., 1999) The child reads a list of 70 exception words until six errors in a row are made Higher Level Reading Tests- Standardiz ed Neale Analysis of Reading Ability, form C (Neale, 1966) Comprehension questions are asked after the child has read a short p a ssa g e Gray Oral Reading Test- Revised (GORT-R) (Wiederhold, & Bryant, 1986) Comprehension subtest- the child reads a passage and answers multiple choice questions Iowa Tests of Basic Skills, Early Primary Battery, Level 6 (Hieronymus, Hoover, & Lindquist, 1986) Measures the child's ability to read words in isolation and to use context and picture cues for word identification. The children were also asked sentence and story comprehension questions Woodcock Reading Mastery Test- form A (WRMT-R) Passage comprehension subtest Diagnostic Achievement Battery- 2 (DAB) (Newcomer, 1990) Reading Comprehension subtest- open-ended questions are asked Silveroli Classroom Reading Inventory- Graded Oral Paragraphs (Silveroli, 1984) The child reads paragraphs aloud with reading mistakes noted. The child is then asked to answer five questions without looking back at the paragraph Stanford Achievement Test, 8th Edition (SAT) (Psychological Corporation, 1992) Reading vocabulary subtest- The child reads a list of words and decides which one of them means the same as an underlined word Reading comprehension subtest- the grade 1 edition measures understanding of simple written sentences and short passages; the grade 3 and grade 7 edition measures the ability to read passages and to answer multiple-choice questions about them 40 Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 Predicting Reading From Oral Language APPENDIX A Oral Language and Reading Measures (continued) Higher level Reading Tests - Standardiz ed (continued) Test of Reading Comprehension (TORC) (Brown, Hammill, & Weiderholt, 1978) Silent reading of passages (no further information available) Some studies included tests that were not oral language measures. These tests are described below. Standardiz ed Tests Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration (Beery, 1982) The child copies geometric forms of increasing complexity Early Childhood Diagnostic Instrument: The Comprehensive Assessment Program (Mason & Stewart, 1989) The 5 subtests used include environmental print in and out of context, story and print concepts, upper and lower case letter naming, beginning and ending word sound awareness and writing Verbal Memory (Weschler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence Sentences; Weschler, 1967) The child repeated sentences gradually increasing in length Nonstandardiz ed Tests Alphabet Recitation Test (Felton & Brown, 1990) The child says the alphabet Finger Localization Test (Felton & Brown, 1990) With the child's hands under a cover, the examiner touches fingers one at a time in a random order and the child identifies which finger was touched by indicating it on a drawing Uppercase and lowercase letter identification (McCormick et al., 1994) First 26 uppercase and then the 26 lower case letters were presented in mixed order and the child was asked to give orally the name for each letter Letter name knowledge (Muter & Snowling, 1998) Child is required to give the name of each letter in the alphabet presented in random order and written in lower case Short-vowel identification test (McCormick et al., 1994) 12 objects were pictured with three words printed under each and the child circled the correct word Spelling test (Snow et al., 1995) The child is required to spell eight words Preschool Reading Achievement (PRA) Parents were asked whether their child could read not at all, a few words, many words, or books Orthographic processing (Badian, 2001) The child points to the one of four stimuli (numbers, letters, and words) that exactly matches the item at the left of the row Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 41 Predicting Reading from Oral Language APPENDIX B. Descriptions of the studies review ed. Bishop and Adams (1990) D e si g n Longitudinal; multiple regression and step-wise multiple regression analyses Participants 83 children whose language development had been impaired at 4 years of age; oral language measures were assessed at ages 4 ½ and 5 ½ and reading abilities assessed at 8 ½ Oral Language measures Expressive phonology, MLU, Bus Story Test, BPVS, TROG, and BAS verbal comprehension and expressive vocabulary subtests Lower level reading measures Reading of non-words and the Neale Analysis of Reading Abilityform C Higher level reading measures Analysis of Reading Ability- form C Felton and Brown (1990) D e si g n Longitudinal; multiple regression analyses Participants 81 children at risk for reading disabilities, 12 repeating kindergarten; oral language skills were assessed in kindergarten and lower level reading was assessed in Grade 1 Oral Language measures Measures entered into the regression analyses: RAN of numbers and letters (combined score), Initial Consonant Not Same, Rhyme, Lindamood Auditory Conceptualization Test, Syllable Counting Test, Metropolitan Readiness Test auditory component, and Otis-Lennon Mental Ability Test Measures not entered into the regression analyses: Final Consonant Different, RAN of colours and objects, Metropolitan Readiness Test visual component and language component, Boston Naming Test, Word String Memory Test, Alphabet Recitation Test, and Finger Localization Test Lower level reading measures WRMT- form A (word identification and word attack subtests) Higher level reading measures N/A Menyuk, Chesnick, Liebergott, Korngold, D'Agostino, and Belanger (1991) 42 D e si g n Longitudinal; step-wise regression analyses Participants 130 children between the ages of 4 ½ and 6 ½ were followed for 3 years; the participants were divided into three groups: 1. Children with specific language impairment (SLI) as determined by standardized tests, 2. at-risk children who had previously been seen for speech and language evaluation, but did not meet the criteria for SLI, and 3. Children who had been born premature weighing less than 1500 grams at birth. Oral Language measures Token Test for Children, TACL, PPVT-R, DSS, Reporters Test, ITPA grammatical closure subtest, Expressive One Word Vocabulary Test, McCarthy Scales of Children's Ability Verbal Fluency subtest, Templin-Darley Articulation Screening Test, TOLD-P auditory discrimination subtest, syllable awareness (syllable segmentation task), phoneme awareness (phoneme segmentation task), word recall, RAN of colours, numbers, letters, and objects, semantactic judgement, cloze, story recall, comprehension of complex sentences. Lower level reading measures sight word recognition subtest of the WRAT and oral reading subtest of the GORT Higher level reading measures silent reading passages from the TORC Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 Predicting Reading From Oral Language APPENDIX B. Descriptions of the studies review ed (continued) Catts 1993 D e si g n Longitudinal; hierarchical fixed-order regression analyses Participants 86 children- 56 speech language impaired and 30 with normal speech-language abilities; oral language measures were first tested in kindergarten and reading abilities were tested in Grade 1 and Grade 2 Oral language measures PPVT-R, Token Test for Children, TOLD-2: P (grammatical understanding, sentence imitation and grammatical closure subtests), Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test, SPELT- II, RAN of colours, objects, and animals, syllable deletion, phoneme deletion, syllable blending, phoneme blending, and Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation (not entered into regression analysis) Lower level reading measures WRMT- form A (word identification and word attack subtests), GORT-R Higher level reading measures GORT-R McCormick, Stoner, and Duncan (1994) D e si g n Longitudinal; multiple regression analyses Participants 38 children of middle socioeconomic whose oral language skills were assessed in kindergarten and whose reading skills were assessed in Grade 1 Oral Language measures PPVT-R, consonant identification task Lower level reading measures Iowa Tests of Basic Skills, Early Primary Battery, Level 6 Higher level reading measures Iowa Tests of Basic Skills, Early Primary Battery, Level 6 Other measures The following predictor variables were used, but are not discussed in this manuscript as they extend beyond oral language skillsDevelopmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration, uppercase and lowercase letter identification, short-vowel identification test Hurford, Schauf, Bunce, Blaich, and Moore (1994) D e si g n Longitudinal; discriminant analysis Participants 171 students followed from beginning of Grade 1 to the end of Grade 2. All language measures were administered four times over the two years to determine if they could predict reader-group membership. Oral Language measures phonological discrimination task, phonemic segmentation of initial and final consonants of real words and pseudowords, PPVT-R Lower level reading measures WRMT-R (word identification and word attack subtests) Higher level reading measures N/A Snow, Tabors, Nicholson, and Kurland (1995) D e si g n Longitudinal; bivariate correlations Participants 84 children from low socioeconomic families tested in kindergarten and again in Grade 1. Oral Language measures PPVT- R, narrative production, picture description, definitions, story comprehension task, superordinates (a subtest of CAP), CELF-R (listening to paragraphs subtest) Lower level reading measures WRAT-R reading subtest, GORT-R Higher level reading measures GORT-R Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 43 Predicting Reading from Oral Language APPENDIX B. Descriptions of the studies review ed (continued) Other measures Subtests from the Early Childhood Diagnostic Instrument: The Comprehensive Assessment Program (scores were tallied and used as an emergent literacy score), spelling test Muter and Snowling (1998) D e si g n Longitudinal; fixed-order multiple regression analyses; discriminant analyses Participants 34 children assessed at ages 4, 5, and 6 to determine if lower level reading skills could be predicted at age 9 Oral Language measures Rhyme detection, deletion task- initial phoneme, and nonword repetition task Lower level reading measures Neale Analysis of Reading Ability- Revised and Graded Nonword Reading Test Higher level reading measures N/A Other measures Letter name knowledge was also a predictor variable O'Connor and Jenkins (1999) D e si g n Longitudinal; discriminant analyses Participants 445 children followed from kindergarten to the end of Grade 1; three cohorts- the first cohort was used to calibrate a model for predicting reading acquisition problems. The second cohort was used to test the reliability of the model and refine scoring criteria. Tests were repeated on the third cohort. Oral Language measures PPVT-R, sound repetition, rapid letter naming, syllable blending, syllable deletion, syllable segmenting, blending phonemes, segmenting phonemes, first sound isolation, rhyme production Lower level reading measures WRMT-R (word identification and word attack subtests) Higher level reading measures WRMT-R (vocabulary and comprehension subtests) Manis, Seidenberg, and Doi (1999) D e si g n Longitudinal; hierarchical regression analyses Participants 67 grade 1 children representing the full range of reading abilities followed for one year until the end of Grade 2 Oral Language measures RAN of letters and numbers, deletion- syllables, deletion- phonemes (the two deletion tasks were combined into one score when entered into the regression analysis) Lower level reading measures WRMT-R (word identification and word attack subtests) and Exception-word reading task Higher level reading measures Silveroli Classroom Reading Inventory- Graded Oral Paragraphs Catts, Fey, Zhang, and Tomblin (1999) and (2001) 44 D e si g n Longitudinal; hierarchical regression analyses (1999); stepwise logistic regression analyses (2001) Participants 604 children were followed from kindergarten and reading was assessed in Grade 2; 328 children had language impairments or nonverbal impairments or both and 276 subjects were typically developing children Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 Predicting Reading From Oral Language APPENDIX B. Descriptions of the studies review ed (continued) Oral Language measures deletion task (syllable and phoneme), RAN of animals, TOLD-2:P (sentence imitation, grammatical completion, picture vocabulary, oral vocabulary and grammatical understanding subtests), Narrative story task Lower level reading measures WRMT-R (word identification and word attack subtests) Higher level reading measures WRMT-R passage comprehension subtest, GORT-R comprehension component, Diagnostic Achievement Battery reading comprehension subtest The authors used data from their 1999 study to predict the likelihood a child in kindergarten will have reading difficulties in Grade 2. Badian (2001) D e si g n Longitudinal; stepwise and hierarchical regression analyses Participants Oral language measures were assessed in kindergarten and reading abilities were assessed in Grades 1, 3, and 7. Ninety-six children participated in Grades kindergarten, 1 and 3. Seventy-nine participants remained by Grade 7. Oral Language measures rhyme detection and syllable segmentation Lower level reading measures Grade 1: SAT (word reading and word study skill subtests) Higher level reading measures Grade 1: SAT (word reading and word study skill subtests) Grade 3: SAT (vocabulary and comprehension subtests) Grade 7: SAT (vocabulary and comprehension subtests) Other measures The following predictive measures were also included in the paper, but are not the focus for this manuscript: parent questionnaire of the amount of words the child could read, verbal IQ, verbal memory, and orthographic processing; the authors also predicted spelling (Grade 7 only) Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 45 Book Review / Évaluation de ressource écrit Book Review / Évaluation de ressource écrit Clinical Education in Speech-Language Pathology Lindy McAllister & Michelle Lincoln (2004) Publisher: Whurr Publishers, London and Philadelphia Available from: The Guilford Press Cost: $51.99 Reviewer: Jeanne Classen, M.A., Head of Clinical Program Affiliation: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders, McGill University, Montreal, QC This is an excellent resource for anyone involved in the clinical education (supervision) of speech-language pathology and audiology¹ students: novice or experienced clinical educators (CEs) (supervisors); students at any level in their clinical education; and university coordinators of clinical education. Both authors are faculty in Australian universities and have doctoral degrees in speech-language pathology and extensive expertise in clinical education. What makes the book both unique and highly appealing is its focus on the voices of CEs and students involved in the clinical education experience. The first-hand accounts of CEs are drawn from McAllister’s doctoral thesis study; the students are heard through the master’s thesis work of one of her students and from the clinical education experiences of both authors. In the preface, the authors explain the central theme of their book, namely “the personal and professional growth that can be achieved by students and clinical educators learning together”. The authors argue for “a humanistic approach to clinical education which is encapsulated in the learning relationships of CEs and students”. The text throughout addresses all involved at various levels of clinical practicum, from one end of the spectrum to the other: from the “novice” student, to the “intermediate”, to the “entry level” student; and from the “beginner” CE, to the “advanced beginner” CE, to the “competent” CE, to the “professional artist”. Some of the authors’ objectives are: to provoke mutual reflection on professional practice and personal development in students and CEs; to provide practical principles and strategies for everyday clinical education situations; and to outline professional development from novice student to professional artistry in the CE. The book consists of eight chapters, each comprising practical learning exercises, checklists, case studies and vignettes, drawn from interviews with CEs and students. Throughout, CEs and students alike are invited, individually or together, to engage in personal reflection, self-assessment, discussion, and problem solving strategies. Chapter 1 explains how growth and 46 development for CEs and students can occur as parallel processes. The underlying assumption is that CEs are not only considered teachers but also life-long learners, and both parties support each other’s learning. The chapter sets out to ask a very basic question: What are the goals for professional development in the clinical education process for both the student and the CE (e.g., continuous development of clinical knowledge and skills)? Next, various models of clinical education are outlined that offer opportunity for professional development for both students and their CEs. The chapter concludes with a description of the stages of professional development in CEs and students. Chapter 2 outlines how the two parties can prepare for the clinical education process. From the CE’s perspective, advantages and disadvantages of taking a student are considered (including motivational aspects for accommodating a student, suitability of the site, appropriate clientele). Students are given advice on how to plan and prepare for a placement (e.g., considering their motivations for accepting a placement; reviewing the goals from the previous placement; studying the orientation package before the next placement). Chapter 3 considers factors that contribute to the development of learning relationships in clinical education. Apart from the usefulness of the self-evaluation and learning exercises, these exercises furnish experienced CEs with a fresh outlook on the learning relationship. The authors further describe some of the problems that may arise in the student-CE relationship and offer suggestions regarding how these can be dealt with. Chapter 4 provides in-depth discussion of the stages of development of personal skills across the spectrum, from the point of view of the novice CE, to the (very) advanced CE. In parallel are described the stages of development of personal skills in the novice student clinician and the intermediate and the competent entry level student. The chapter also deals with issues such as using assertive communication, avoiding or managing emotional labour, and preventing or dealing with burnout in clinical education. Chapter 5 discusses the development of cognitive skills. It describes four areas of cognitive skill, namely: 1) different types of knowledge (theoretical, practical, personal, tacit); 2) different approaches to reasoning; 3) clinical reasoning; and 4) ethical reasoning. Of these, clinical reasoning may be of particular interest to the reader, even if not involved in clinical education. Our professions are introduced to the relatively little known concept of clinical reasoning – in contrast to many other rehab and medical professions where this is a well established concept. The authors advance that clinical reasoning is not much used in speechlanguage pathology due, in part, to the focus on problem solving (i.e. outcome) rather than on the process of thinking about problems. The vignettes and learning exercises in this chapter illustrate with specific examples how different types of reasoning can be engaged in and Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 Book Review / Évaluation de ressource écrit how these skills can be applied in specific situations. Chapter 6 describes the learning processes that can be applied in clinical education. Reflection is considered an important learning process for students and CEs alike, as well as for clinicians not engaged in student training. Students are encouraged and given suggestions on how to reflect on activities, such as producing treatment plans, report/note writing, journaling and supervision conferencing. Clinicians are invited to reflect on areas such as ethical dilemmas, case presentations and team meetings. Chapter 7 discusses how learning can be assessed in clinical settings and how learning can be facilitated through assessment. Different types of assessment terminology are described (e.g., formative versus summative assessment). The importance of selfassessment for students and CEs and the need for each party to assess the other are stressed. The final chapter outlines a plan for ongoing skill development in clinical education. It outlines the professional development for all levels of CEs and students. Suggestions are given in regard to the different responsibilities that can be undertaken by CEs with different levels of expertise. For example, more experienced CEs may mentor new colleagues or try innovative clinical education models. The authors further stress the need for raising the status and recognition of CEs by their employers, universities and professional associations. This is followed by a number of strategies for dealing with this issue (e.g., provision of funding and resources, recognition of excellence, putting in place creative placements that benefit students and organizations). This chapter also discusses the role and responsibilities of universities in preparing students for clinical placements and providing training and support for CEs. The text devoted to the development of professional and personal skills would be arguably somewhat lengthy or redundant in places were it not for its original focus, namely the first-hand accounts of CEs and students. This allows the reader, whether a student or a CE, an opportunity to become acquainted not only with his or her own learning processes and some of the challenges involved and strategies to deal with these, but also those of the other party. With the book’s emphases on personal/ professional skill building on the one hand and development of clinical reasoning skills on the other, one clinical education model that could have comfortably nestled inside this text is missing, in my opinion. I refer to the “two-to-one supervision” model or “reciprocal peer coaching” model where two students simultaneously undertake a clinical practicum with one CE. The recent clinical reasoning literature (which has emerged largely from Australia) has reported on the enhanced learning outcomes that may result when two students during a two-to-one practicum have ongoing opportunity to engage together in clinical reasoning, with or without the presence of their CE. Employing such a clinical education model may give a new impetus to the more experienced CE or professional artist. The chapter on assessment (i.e., feedback in its many forms) is very informative, and provides all levels of CEs with information for adopting in their feedback and evaluation sessions with students. Strategies are offered on how CEs can teach students to self-assess, or what to do if the CE’s assessment of the student does not match the student’s self-assessment. Students are given tools for self-assessment, such as videotaping and journaling. This book is highly recommended for speechlanguage pathologists and audiologists with or without any background or experience in clinical education. University coordinators of clinical education will find this textbook a rich resource when preparing students for clinical practicum and for developing workshops for CEs. One of the major strengths and attractions of this easy-to-read book that abounds in practical ideas is that it includes throughout the perspective of both the CE and the student. Aside from making it a very useful resource for both these groups, it thus allows each party to see the other’s perspective throughout the learning process, which in turn is likely to lead to greater mutual appreciation and understanding. In this way, the work has lived up to its premise to espouse the humanistic approach, which should particularly appeal to our communication-based professions. ________________________________________________________________________ ¹ Although this book is not specifically addressed to audiologists, it is recommended for other health care professionals, and indeed, most, if not all of the content, may equally apply to audiologists. The most likely reason why audiologists are not explicitly mentioned is that in Australia the two professions are not associated. Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 47 Book Review / Évaluation de ressource écrit Book Review / Évaluation de ressource écrit Phonological Awareness: From research to practice Gail T. Gillon (2004) Publisher: The Guilford Press: New York, NY Cost: $35.00 US Reviewer: Susan Rvachew, Ph.D., S-LP(C), Assistant Professor Affiliation: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders, McGill University, Montreal, QC This book was written for professionals and students who are responsible for helping children who are at risk or experiencing difficulties with the acquisition of reading and spelling skills. It is intended to help the reader understand phonological awareness and its role in the development of reading and spelling. Furthermore, it is intended to make explicit the path from research to practice by providing a framework for the accurate identification and successful resolution of phonological awareness deficits. The book is comprised of ten chapters. The first defines the construct of phonological awareness and describes the tasks that are used to measure it at the syllable, onsetrime, and phoneme levels. The next three chapters are focused on reading and spelling development, with the second reviewing models of literacy acquisition, the third discussing the role of phonological awareness in reading development, and the fourth describing the phonological awareness skills of children with dyslexia. The fifth chapter discusses the phonological awareness skills of children with specific language impairment, articulation disorders, phonological delay of unknown origin and dyspraxia of speech. Chapters six through nine are focused on clinical practice, describing assessment tools, instructional frameworks, and some of the activities that can be used to remediate deficits in phonological awareness for children of different ages. The final chapter, with sections written by Sally Clendon, Linda Cupples, Mark Flynn, Teresa Iacono, Traci Schmidtkie, David Yoder, and Audrey Young, briefly reviews the literature relating to the phonological awareness skills of children with physical, sensory, or intellectual impairments. This book is a very good resource for any professional who is working with children who are at risk for phonological awareness deficits. The review of the research evidence is comprehensive but readable. The right balance between breadth and depth of coverage is maintained throughout the book. Individual studies are described with just enough detail to allow the reader to fully understand the findings and conclusions (although the author’s evaluation of the quality of the studies is somewhat shallow as described below). The implications of the research literature for clinical and educational 48 practice are made explicit at the end of each chapter. Informative case examples appear throughout the book. The two chapters on intervention do not provide a stepby-step ‘how-to’ guide to the remediation of phonological awareness deficits. Rather, these chapters emphasise guiding principles that should underlay the development of a comprehensive intervention program that is customized to meet the needs and interests of each individual client. Some specific intervention activities are described but the clinician is advised to continually monitor the client’s progress and adapt the activities accordingly. This book would also be appropriate as a text book for a senior undergraduate or graduate level course on phonological awareness. As with any text book, however, the instructor would need to be thoroughly familiar with the background literature in order to compensate for some of the weaknesses of the literature review. The primary weakness of the book is that the links drawn between research and practice are more intuitive than systematic. The author fails to explicitly apply the principles of evidence-based practice when helping the reader use the research evidence to guide clinical practice. (More information about the process of evidence-based clinical decision making can be found on the ASHA website1). A particularly important aspect of evidencebased decision making is the necessity of evaluating rather than simply summarizing the available research. This failure to evaluate the research evidence is apparent in some of the unresolved issues that reoccur throughout the book. For example, the literature relating to the relationship between rime awareness and reading acquisition is, on the surface, highly confusing, leaving the clinician uncertain about whether to teach rime awareness to a child with delayed phonological awareness skills. In order to make sense of the conflicting conclusions of researchers who have investigated this relationship, it is necessary to consider the psychometric properties of the tests used and the quality of the research designs employed. For example, correlational studies in which an unreliable measure of rime awareness yields a restricted range of test outcomes by the participants should be discounted. Unfortunately, this level of analysis is curiously lacking in much of the book and some studies with glaring weaknesses are cited repeatedly (the final chapter is an exception as it contains some nice examples of appropriate evaluation of the quality of evidence). Another unresolved issue concerns the number of different skills that should be taught within the context of a phonological awareness intervention. Some programs recommend a dizzying array of target skills while others focus on one or two core skills, such as segmenting words into phonemes. Specific guidelines for evaluating the quality of evidence have been proposed and could have been applied in an effort to both model this decision-making process and to answer the question Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 Book Review / Évaluation de ressource écrit about the optimum number of target skills. Unfortunately the author appears to credit her own study (in which a non-experimental, self-selected control group was employed) more highly than the meta-analyses that have examined the efficacy of phonological awareness interventions. Non-experimental studies can make very valuable contributions, especially when establishing the feasibility of a treatment approach early in the history of a research program. However, randomised control trials and meta-analyses constitute the strongest evidence that can be brought to bear on questions of relative efficacy of competing treatment practices. This criticism notwithstanding, the book is still valuable as a textbook and would provide an opportunity for the instructor to demonstrate the use of evidence-based decision making to resolve some of the conflicting findings that emerge from the literature that is summarized by Gillon. In summary this book would be a valuable resource for practicing clinicians and educators as well as a useful textbook for students who expect to help children who may have difficulties with phonological awareness. It provides a valuable introduction for readers who are new to this topic as well as a useful quick reference for those who are more familiar with this large literature. 1 ASHA members can access the Technical Report entitled ‘Evidence-Based Practice in Communication Disorders: An Introduction’ at www.asha.org. Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 49 Materials Review / Évaluation de ressource écrit Resource Reviews / Évaluation des ressources Pre-Reading Inventory of Phonological Awareness B. Dodd, S. Crosbie, B. McIntosh, T. Teitzel, and A. Ozanne (2003) Publisher: The Psychological Corporation, 19500 Bulverde Road, San Antonio, TX 78259 www.PsychCorp.com Cost: $153.00 (US) Reviewed by: Lu-Anne McFarlane, Associate Professor Affiliation: Department of Speech Pathology and Audiology, University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB The Pre-Reading Inventory of Phonological Awareness is designed to assess six areas of phonological awareness development in children age 4 years, 0 months through 6 years, 11 months. In the younger age range (pre-kindergarten and early kindergarten), it is intended as a baseline of phonological awareness skills. For the older age group, it can be used to identify those with phonological awareness deficits. The six areas tested are: Rhyme Awareness, Syllable Segmentation, Alliteration Awareness, Sound Isolation, Sound Segmentation, and Letter-Sound Knowledge. All sub-tests include clear administration instructions within the stimulus book. All of them also include demonstration items that allow feedback on response accuracy. Some also include trial items. For the sub-tests with pictures, the illustrations are simple, colored and age appropriate. The testing protocol allows the examiner to discontinue testing after a prescribed number of errors on three of the sub-tests; the rest need to be administered in their entirety. The sub-tests for Rhyme Awareness and Alliteration Awareness are done in an “odd one out” format. This format presents the child with four words and asks them to identify which “doesn’t belong”. The four items are pictured, which reduces the load on short-term memory. However, the odd one out format is a more complex response than identifying a word that does rhyme or alliterate. This can pose challenges in measuring rhyme or alliteration skills in the younger age range, where the complexity of the response type may prevent demonstration of emerging skills. The Syllable Segmentation and Sound Segmentation tasks are scored on the basis of the child’s ability to orally segment the word into syllables or sounds. For Syllable Segmentation, the instructions ask the child to “clap out” the syllables as he or she says the word with clear syllable boundaries, or to point to drums on the page while segmenting the word orally. The demonstration word 50 (elephant) is pictured, but none of the 4 trial words or test words is pictured. None of the test words used for Syllable Segmentation is likely to be in the vocabulary of the target age for the test (abyss, periodical, magnitude, elaboration). This fact, combined with the lack of pictures, would definitely place a load on short-term memory for 4 to 6 year olds, confounding any results. In the Sound Segmentation task, the child is asked to orally segment the word into sounds, using counters as a support. No picture support is provided but the target words are appropriate for the target age (spoon, shoe, lady, cake). The Sound Isolation task asks the child to identify the first sound in a pictured word. All of the words are appropriate vocabulary items for the target age range. The Letter-Sound task provides the child with a grapheme (or graphemes) in print and asks the child to identify what sound the letters make. Digraphs, vowels and clusters are included. Raw scores from each subtest are converted to percentile ranges. These ranges are in 5-percentile increments. Additionally, there are often large jumps in percentile ranks for only a small raw score difference. For example, in the conversion chart for 5 year olds taking the Alliteration Awareness subtest, a raw score of 1 places the child in the 0 – 4th percentile, a score of 2 results in a percentile of 10 – 14, and a raw score of 3 places the child in the 30 – 34th percentile. The percentiles are then grouped into three categories: Emerging/Below Basic (0 – 29th percentile), Basic (30th to 69th percentile), and Proficient (70th to 99th percentile). This grouping blurs important distinctions in performance. The Emerging category is used for the 4 and 5 year olds, rather than labeling them as Below Basic. The authors recommend that those scoring in the Below Basic or in the low end of Basic receive intervention focused on phonological awareness skills. The manual includes several case studies as examples in interpretation. The standardization sample for this test included 450 children stratified by variables such as ethnicity, geographic region and parent education. Interestingly, 11% of the standardization sample had some area of educational or developmental concern, including speech and language disorder/delay, learning disability, and developmental delay. A wide range of reliability and validity analyses were undertaken, indicating general support for the test. The Pre-Reading Inventory of Phonological Awareness test can provide the speech-language pathologist or classroom teacher with information on development of phonological awareness. The strengths of this test are the clear instructions in the stimulus book and ease of scoring. The subtests most suitable for the 4 and 5 year olds (Rhyme Awareness, Alliteration Awareness and Syllable Segmentation) have complex response types, so are unlikely to be sensitive to early stages of development. Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 The Canadian Association of Speech-Language Pathologists and Audiologists Position Paper on the Use of Telepractice for CASLPA Speech-Language Pathologists and Audiologists POSITION: The Canadian Association of Speech-Language Pathologists and Audiologists (CASLPA) endorses the use of telepractice in both speech-language pathology and audiology as a means of improving access to services provided by fully qualified professionals. CASLPA members who provide services via telepractice are expected to adhere to the CASLPA Code of Ethics (2005) and privacy legislation and abide by guidelines established by the regulatory body of the province in which they reside. Members may provide services via telepractice if they determine that it is in the best interests of their clients. Members shall base telepractice on best evidence and encourage ongoing research in this field. DEFINITIONS: “Client” refers to the individual receiving professional services and in the case of an individual who is not capable, the legal guardian or legal representative. With the consent of the individual or of the legal guardian/representative, “client” may also include family members and caregivers. “Informed consent” means a client gives consent to receive a proposed service following a process of decision-making leading to an informed choice. Valid consent may be either verbal or written unless otherwise required by institutional or provincial/territorial regulation. The client is provided with sufficient information, including the benefits and risks, and the possible alternatives to the proposed service, and the client understands this information. The client can withdraw informed consent at any time. “Member” used throughout the document refers to a speech-language pathologist or audiologist who is a full member of the Canadian Association of Speech-Language Pathologists and Audiologists (CASLPA). “Provider site” refers to the site where the member providing service is physically located. “Recipient site” refers to the site where the client is located. “Telepractice” refers to the use of technology to deliver audiology and speech–language pathology services at a distance. Telepractice may involve “live” or “store-and-forward” service. Live or real time service may include but is not limited to telephone, or videoconferencing. Store and forward involves the recording, storing, and subsequent transmission of audio and/or visual images for later examination (e.g., e-mail, fax, audiotape or videotape recordings). RATIONALE: a) Advances in technology have allowed professionals in various fields including speech-language pathologists and audiologists to utilize telepractice. It is therefore important that members have guidelines for minimum standards for telepractice. b) The challenges of travel distance and cost, weather, mobility and disruption of family and work schedules, can impede access to services. Telepractice may help overcome such challenges by: • increasing the frequency of contact and efficiency of service; • increasing accessibility of services for clients; • providing members with access to continuing professional development opportunities. c) Telepractice can aid collaborative work by: • providing members with access to consultation with those with more specialized expertise; • providing members with access to team services where other team members are separated by distance. d) Telepractice may help maximize the use of available supports in the client’s area by: • affording members opportunities for educating caregivers and service providers in the client’s community; • accessing the services of interpreters to provide services to clients in their native language. Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 51 RECOMMENDATIONS/GUIDELINES FOR CASLPA SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGISTS AND AUDIOLOGISTS: 1. Members shall be aware of and accountable to the appropriate jurisdiction(s) and any/all scope of practice requirements, particularly with respect to provider and recipient sites. Applicable jurisdiction(s) shall be consulted for details. 2. The scope and nature of activities conducted via telepractice shall be comparable to that provided during an inperson session. The quality of audio and video signals must be appropriate for the activity being conducted. Alterations to standard in-person clinical protocols should be documented (e.g. explain and document how oral motor or otoscopic examinations were conducted and adapted, identify any limitations in interpretation). 3. Members shall ensure that: • they are competent in the use of the equipment and/or that the appropriate technological assistance is • • • available to them; all equipment is operational and calibrated (if applicable); they comply with applicable safety laws, regulations and codes; relevant infection control policies and procedures are followed. 4. Members shall be aware that the success of services provided through telepractice may be influenced by cultural beliefs. Comfort levels of participants may vary depending on previous experience. 5. The member providing the service via telepractice shall protect the privacy and confidentiality of the client with the knowledge that some forms of transmission are more secure than others and that unauthorized persons may access exchanges and/or information. Members shall inform clients that safeguards have been established to protect privacy and confidentiality but that no technological communication system is entirely secure. Members shall identify all attendees in both provider and recipient sites. 6. Members shall ensure that informed consent is obtained prior to the telepractice session. Informed consent shall encompass such information as the advantages and limitations of the mode of service, alternative service options, use and storage of transmitted signals, plan of action in the event of technology failure and who is responsible for on-going care. 7. The responsibility for fee reimbursement shall be established prior to the telepractice service if applicable. 8. The member in the recipient site shall be responsible for the main care of the client unless alternative arrangements have been made. 9. Provincial/territorial professional bodies should expand on these telepractice guidelines to ensure that practice standards are in accordance with provincial/territorial regulations. 10. This position statement shall be reviewed within two (2) years or sooner as necessary. Consideration should be given to evolving technologies, resources and requirements for increasing competencies. Background Background: The CASLPA Ad Hoc Committee on telepractice was formed in the fall of 2003 in response to needs expressed by CASLPA members. The mandate of this committee was to develop a position statement that identified issues concerning the role of speech-language pathology and audiology members in the use of telepractice, and to establish professional practice guidelines for providing services via telepractice. The committee was comprised of speech-language pathologists and audiologists with experience, interest, and expertise in issues related to service delivery in the area of telepractice. This position paper was developed following review of literature, relevant position statements and guidelines, dialogue and consultation. Suggested Reading: National Initiative for Telehealth (NIFTE): National Initiative for Telehealth (NIFTE) Framework of Guidelines. September 2003, Ottawa; NIFTE www.nifte.ca 52 Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 References: American Speech-Language-Hearing Association: Telepractices and ASHA: Report of the Telepractices Team, December 2001: www.asha.org. American Speech-Language-Hearing Association (2005) Speech-language pathologists providing clinical services via telepractice: Position Statement. ASHA Supplement 25, in press. American Speech-Language-Hearing Association.(2005) Audiologists providing clinical services via telepractice: Position statement. ASHA Supplement 25, in press. American Speech-Language-Hearing Association.(2005) Audiologists providing clinical services via telepractice: Technical report. ASHA Supplement 25, in press. American Speech-Language-Hearing Association.(2005) Speech-language pathologists providing clinical services via telepractice: Technical Report. ASHA Supplement 25, in press. Canadian Association of Occupational Therapists: CAOT Position Statement Tele-Health and Teleoccupational Therapy (2000) http://www.caot.ca/default.asp?ChangeID=190&pageID=187. Canadian Association of Speech-Language Pathologists and Audiologists (2005). Code of Ethics. : http://www.caslpa.ca/english/resources/ethics.asp. Canadian Physicians and Surgeons of Manitoba Telehealth Guidelines and Statements, www.cpsm.mb.ca/guidelines and statements/166.html. College of Audiologists and Speech-Language Pathologists of Ontario (CASLPO), Position Paper: Use of Telepractice Approaches in Providing Services To Patients/ Clients.Toronto,ApprovedJune2004 http://www.caslpo.com/english_site/mptelepractice.doc. College of Occupational Therapists of Ontario, Telepractice: Information for Occupational Therapists Providing Telehealth Services, 2001. http://www.coto.org/media/documents/Telepractice_brochure.pdf. National Initiative for Telehealth (NIFTE): National Initiative for Telehealth (NIFTE) Framework of Guidelines. September 2003 ,Ottawa; NIFTE www.nifte.ca. Steinecke, Richard: Regulating Telepractice, May 2002-no 51 Grey Areas, Steinecke Maciura Leblanc, Publications and Newsletter, www.sml-law.com. Committee members: Candace Myers, MSc, S-LP(C), Chair Patricia Carey, M.Ed., S-LP(C), Registered SK Alvilda Douglas, M.Sc., Aud(C) Sean Kinden, M.A., Aud(C) Deborah Kully, M.S., R.SLP, S-LP(C), CCC-SLP, BRS-FD Ariane Laplante-Levesque, MPA, MSc, Aud(C) Wendy MacDonald, M.Sc.(A), S-LP(C) Mary Pole, S-LP(C) Carrie Stacey, M.Sc., S-LP(C) Karen Svitich, MSLP, R.SLP, S-LP(C) Acknowledgements: The Committee wishes to thank Morley Hewison, Provincial Network Operations Manager of Telehealth Saskatchewan for his input, and Sharon Fotheringham of CASLPA for her guidance and support throughout this project. A position paper represents the direction CASLPA has taken on a particular topic or provides guidelines for particular areas of practice. These positions are time-bound, representing the thinking at a particular point in time. Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 53 L’Association Canadienne des Orthophonistes et Audiologistes Énoncé de position sur l’utilisation de la télépratique par les orthophonistes et les audiologistes membres de l’ACOA Position: L’Association canadienne des orthophonistes et audiologistes (ACOA) appuie le recours à la télépratique tant dans le domaine de l’orthophonie que de l’audiologie en vue d’améliorer l’accès à des services prodigués par des professionnels compétents. Les membres de l’ACOA qui offrent des services par le biais de la télépratique doivent respecter le Code de déontologie de l’ACOA (2005), la législation relative à la protection de la vie privée ainsi que les lignes directrices établies par l’organisme de réglementation de la province où ils se trouvent. Les membres peuvent fournir des services par le biais de la télépratique s’ils jugent que cela est dans l’intérêt supérieur de leur client. Ils doivent fonder leur exercice de la télépratique sur des données probantes et encourager la poursuite de la recherche dans ce domaine. Définitions Définitions: L’expression « client » renvoie à une personne qui bénéficie de services professionnels et, dans le cas d’une personne inapte, à son tuteur légal ou à son représentant légal. Avec le consentement de la personne ou de son tuteur/représentant légal, l’expression « client » peut aussi inclure les membres de la famille et les fournisseurs de soins. L’expression « consentement éclairé » renvoie au consentement que donne un client qui accepte en toute connaissance de cause de recevoir un service proposé. Un consentement valide peut être verbal ou écrit, à moins que la réglementation institutionnelle, provinciale ou territoriale ne stipule autrement. Le client doit être bien informé, y compris des avantages et des risques ainsi que des solutions de rechange au service proposé. Il doit comprendre ces renseignements. Il peut retirer son consentement éclairé à n’importe quel moment. L’expression « membre » renvoie aux orthophonistes et aux audiologistes membres en règle de l’Association canadienne des orthophonistes et audiologistes (ACOA). L’expression « emplacement du fournisseur » renvoie à l’endroit où se situe le membre qui offre le service. L’expression « emplacement du bénéficiaire » renvoie à l’endroit où se situe le client. L’expression « télépratique » renvoie à l’utilisation de la technologie pour offrir des services d’orthophonie et d’audiologie à distance. La télépratique peut comprendre un service « en direct » ou « en différé ». Le mode « en direct » ou en temps réel fait appel notamment au téléphone ou à la vidéoconférence. Le mode « en différé » nécessite l’enregistrement, la sauvegarde et la transmission d’images audio ou visuelles qui seront examinées ultérieurement (p. ex. : courriel, télécopie, bande sonore ou enregistrements magnétoscopiques). Fondement : a) Les percées technologiques permettent à des professionnels de différentes disciplines, y compris l’orthophonie et l’audiologie, de recourir à la télépratique. Pour cette raison, il est important que les membres disposent de lignes directrices qui établissent des normes minimales pour la télépratique. b) Les difficultés et les coûts qu’entraînent de longs déplacements, le climat, la mobilité et les dérangements pour la famille et le travail sont autant de facteurs qui nuisent à l’accès à des services. Le recours à la télépratique permet de surmonter une partie de ces obstacles, car elle : • accroît la fréquence du suivi et l’efficacité du service • améliore l’accessibilité des services pour les clients • offre aux membres l’accès à des occasions des activités de perfectionnement. c) La télépratique peut favoriser la collaboration, car elle : • offre aux membres la possibilité de consulter une personne qui possède des compétences spécialisées • offre aux membres la possibilité de travailler en équipe, même si l’un des membres de l’équipe se situe à distance. d) La télépratique aide à maximiser l’utilisation des services de soutien dans la région du client, car elle : • donne aux membres l’occasion de former des fournisseurs de soins et des fournisseurs de service dans la collectivité du client • accède aux services d’interprètes afin d’offrir des services à des clients dans leur langue maternelle. 54 Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 Recommandations/Lignes directrices pour les orthophonistes et audiologistes membres de l’ACOA : 1. Les membres doivent connaître et respecter les compétences appropriées ainsi que toutes les exigences visant la pratique, notamment en ce qui a trait à l’emplacement du fournisseur et du bénéficiaire. Il faut consulter toutes les compétences pour connaître leurs exigences. 2. Le champ et la nature des activités menées par le biais de la télépratique doivent être comparables à ceux durant les séances en personne. La qualité des signaux audio et vidéo doit convenir pour les activités menées. Toute déviation au protocole habituel pour les rencontres privées en clinique doit être consignée par écrit (p. ex. : expliquer et documenter comment s’est déroulé l’examen moteur et otoscopique et comment il a été adapté, préciser les limites imposées à l’interprétation des résultats). 3. Les membres doivent s’assurer que : • ils ont les compétences nécessaires pour utiliser l’équipement ou qu’ils disposent de l’aide technique nécessaire • tout le matériel fonctionne et qu’il est calibré (s’il y a lieu) • ils se conforment aux lois, règlements et codes visant la sécurité • ils respectent les politiques et méthodes pertinentes pour la prévention des infections. 4. Les membres doivent savoir que la réussite des services offerts par le biais de la télépratique peut être tributaire des croyances culturelles. Le sentiment d’aise des participants variera selon leur expérience. 5. Les membres offrant un service par le biais de la télépratique doivent protéger la vie privée et les renseignements personnels du client. Il faut savoir que certaines formes de transmission sont plus sécurisées que d’autres et que des personnes non autorisées pourraient avoir accès aux échanges et à des renseignements. Les membres doivent aviser le client que des mesures de protection sont en place pour préserver la confidentialité des renseignements personnels, mais qu’aucune technologie n’est infaillible. Les membres doivent présenter toutes les personnes présentes tant à l’emplacement du fournisseur qu’à celui du bénéficiaire. Les membres doivent veiller à obtenir un consentement éclairé avant les séances de télépratique. Pour ce faire, ils doivent expliquer les avantages et les limites d’un mode de prestation de services, les services de rechange, l’utilisation et le stockage des signaux transmis, le plan d’action en cas de défaillance technique et la personne qui sera responsable des soins de suivi. 6. 7. Il faut établir avant le début des services de télépratique qui assumera le remboursement des frais afférents. 8. Il incombe aux membres à l’emplacement du bénéficiaire d’assurer l’essentiel des soins du client à moins que d’autres dispositions n’aient été prises. Les organismes provinciaux et territoriaux devraient se fonder sur ces lignes directrices visant la télépratique pour s’assurer que les normes de pratique respectent la réglementation provinciale ou territoriale. 9. 10. Le présent énoncé de position doit être révisé d’ici deux (2) ans ou plus tôt selon les besoins. Il faudra tenir compte des nouvelles technologies, des ressources disponibles et de l’évolution des besoins afin d’améliorer les compétences. Mise en contexte: Le comité spécial de l’ACOA sur la télépratique a vu le jour à l’automne 2003 en réaction à des besoins exprimés par les membres de l’ACOA. Ce comité avait pour mandat d’élaborer un énoncé de position pour cerner les questions entourant le recours à la télépratique par les orthophonistes et audiologistes membres et pour établir des lignes directrices visant la prestation de services par le biais de la télépratique. Ce comité se composait d’orthophonistes et d’audiologistes ayant l’expérience de la télépratique, des intérêts en la matière et l’expertise de la prestation de services par ce moyen. Pour élaborer cet énoncé de position, les membres du comité ont passé en revue la documentation sur le sujet ainsi que les énoncés de position pertinents; ils ont dialogué et ont tenu des consultations. Lecture proposée: National Initiative for Telehealth (NIFTE). National Initiative for Telehealth (NIFTE) Framework of Guidelines. Septembre 2003, Ottawa, www.nifte.ca. Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 55 Références: American Speech-Language-Hearing Association (décembre 2001). Telepractices and ASHA: Report of the Telepractices Team, www.asha.org. American Speech-Language-Hearing Association (2005). Speech-language pathologists providing clinical services via telepractice: Position Statement. ASHA Supplement 25, sous presse. American Speech-Language-Hearing Association(2005). Audiologists providing clinical services via telepractice: Position statement. ASHA Supplement 25, sous presse. American Speech-Language-Hearing Association.(2005). Audiologists providing clinical services via telepractice: Technical report. ASHA Supplement 25, sous presse. American Speech-Language-Hearing Association.(2005) Speech-language pathologists providing clinical services via telepractice: Technical Report. ASHA Supplement 25, sous presse. Association canadienne des ergothérapeutes (2000). Prise de position révisée sur les services de télé-ergothérapie de qualité, www.caot.ca/default.asp?ChangeID=190&pageID=187. Association canadienne des orthophonistes et audiologistes (2005). Code de déontologie, www.caslpa.ca/francais/resources/ethics.asp. Canadian Physicians and Surgeons of Manitoba Telehealth Guidelines and Statements, www.cpsm.mb.ca/guidelines and statements/166.html National Initiative for Telehealth (septembre 2003). National Initiative for Telehealth Framework of Guidelines. Ottawa, NIFTE, www.nifte.ca. Ordre des audiologistes et des orthophonistes de l’Ontario (juin 2004). Énoncé de principe : Utilisation d’approches de télépratique pour la prestation de services aux patients ou clients, Toronto, www.caslpo.com/french_site/m_memposit.asp. Ordre des ergothérapeutes de l’Ontario (2001). Télémédecine : renseignements à l’intention des ergothérapeutes qui offrent des services de télémédecine. www.coto.org/media/ documents/Telepractice_brochure_fr.pdf. Steinecke, Richard (mai 2002). Regulating Telepractice no 51 Grey Areas, Steinecke Maciura Leblanc, Publications and Newsletter, www.sml-law.com. Membres du comité : Candace Myers, MSc, O(C), présidente Patricia Carey, M.Ed., O(C), Registered SK Alvilda Douglas, M.Sc., Aud(C) Sean Kinden, M.A., Aud(C) Deborah Kully, M.S., R.SLP, O(C), CCC-SLP, BRS-FD Ariane Laplante-Levesque, MPA, MSc, Aud(C) Wendy MacDonald, M.Sc.(A), O(C) Mary Pole, O(C) Carrie Stacey, M.Sc., O(C) Karen Svitich, MSLP, R.SLP, O(C) Remerciements: Le comité souhaite remercier Morley Hewison, directeur de l’exploitation provinciale des réseaux pour ses suggestions et Sharon Fotheringham de l’ACOA pour ses conseils et son soutien tout au long de cette initiative. Un énoncé de position représente l’orientation adoptée par l’ACOA concernant un sujet particulier ou fournit des lignes directrices visant un aspect quelconque de la pratique. Ces énoncés ont une durée limitée et représente le raisonnement d’un moment précis. 56 Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 CASLPA 2006 Abstracts CASLPA 2006 Abstr a c ts Winnip anit o ba innipee g , M Manit anito M a y 3-6, 2006 Preconference Workshops Tracheostomy and Ventilator Dependency Management of Breathing, Speaking and Swallowing in Adults Donna Tippett, Departments of Otolaryngology – Head and Neck Surgery, and Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MA Management of speaking and swallowing disorders in adults with tracheostomy/ventilator dependency is challenging. This workshop will be presented at a basic-intermediate level to prepare speech-language pathologists to evaluate and treat speaking and swallowing disorders in this population. Workshop content will also be relevant to occupational therapists, physical therapists, nurses, and respiratory therapists. The SCERTS Model: A Comprehensive, Multidisciplinary Educational Approach for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders Emily Rubin, Director, Communication Crossroads, Carmel, CA This workshop will outline the SCERTS model, a comprehensive, multidisciplinary educational approach designed to enhance the communication and socio-emotional abilities of children with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) while providing family support and support to professionals. The acronym “SCERTS” refers to the three primary dimensions of the model. Developmental objectives in Social Communication (SC) and Emotional Regulation (ER) are addressed by implementing Transactional Supports (TS) (e.g., interpersonal style adjustments, environmental arrangements, visual supports, etc.) throughout a child’s daily activities and across social partners in order to facilitate competence within these identified goal areas in functional and meaningful contexts (e.g., home, school, and community). The presentation will begin with a review of current treatment efficacy literature and the recommended guidelines for educational programming put forth by the National Academy of Sciences in the United States. This discussion will then be followed with practical guidelines for implementation, as illustrated through video case reviews. Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology Workshop Coaching: Unleashing Individual and Service Potential Jim McLaren, Winnipeg, MB Forward thinking practitioners understand that relationships are the real and future “currency” of the work world and that coaching skills can help develop those relationships. Coaching works on the belief that it is the client who ultimately has the answers for any situation, and coaching can assist in uncovering those answers. Alison Hendron, director of training for the Certified Executive Coaching program at Royal Roads University, describes coaching this way: “You take bike riding: a consultant explains the different bikes and recommends the best one for you; a therapist helps you overcome your fear of falling, while the executive coach runs along beside you to make sure you’re steady before you ride away on your own.” This plenary session is designed to share insights on the power of a coaching approach and the potential it offers for those in clinical practice settings. I Used to Have a Handle on Life, but It Broke Shari Robertson, Phoenix Enterprises, Inc., Indiana, PA This session provides proven strategies for managing time and stress specific to professionals within the field of communicative disorders. This is a fast-paced seminar with lots of audience participation and lots of laughter. Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 57 CASLPA 2006 Abstracts Third Party/Insurance Coverage – Demystifying the World of Insurance and Third Party Payers Katherine Cheney, Stewart McIntosh, Veterans Affairs Canada; Tania Stote, Blue Cross Medavie; Jocelyne Lavergene-Robenhymer, First Nations and Inuit Health Branch, Non-Insured Health Benefits (NIHB); Michael O’Brien, Canada Revenue Agency: Disability Tax Credit This session will provide you with the opportunity to hear from Veterans Affairs Canada, the Workman’s Compensation Board, Non-Insured Health Benefits, Blue Cross, and other third party payers and insurance companies on their policies and funding procedures. You will be provided with the opportunity to ask specific questions of each of the individuals presenting. Think Critically!: What to Look for in the Research You Read Lisa Lix, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, MB Evidence-based decision-making is a popular catch phrase. How can you effectively use research evidence to make decisions in your job? This presentation will focus on the three key things you should “get” from a critical research review: (1) get the message: What are the main findings of this research? (2) get the methods: Are the conclusions justified based on the data and analyses?, and (3) get the big picture: Are the results of one study relevant to the clients I see each day? Universal Design Teaching Strategies Karen Priestley, Winnipeg, MB Universal design is an exciting new framework facilitating new ways to meet the needs of our diverse student population. It is a movement towards actualizing inclusion in its truest sense as it looks at students in terms of needs and does not adhere to disability categorization. Universal design emphasizes the goal of student access to the general curriculum for all of our children. It involves a paradigm shift from adapting instruction, curriculum and assessment to meet diverse or exceptional student needs as they arise (inclusion), to using great foresight to expect and plan for all needs as the norm (universal design). It is not a radically new way of teaching but extends many of our current methodologies and is built on what people in the field of education already know from research and practice about good teaching and creating accessible environments. Universal design is all-encompassing and involves all members of the learning community. The theoretical underpinnings of the universal design concept emerged from the field of architecture and have been applied to education since around 1990. Universal design is particularly associated with the field of special education as philosophically applying the universal design concept to education means that the general curriculum is readily accessible to all students regardless of varied learning needs. Implementation of universal design involves restructuring, creativity and collaboration. A Coach Approach Jim McLaren, Winnipeg, MB This workshop will provide a dynamic, experience-based approach to learning. The day is designed for maximum flexibility to address the “pain points” and meet the needs and interests of individual participants. It includes an emphasis on group discussion and draws from the collective wisdom and experiences of participants. Activities include such learning techniques as case studies, practice exercises, role plays, demonstrations, self awareness and “team” assignments. At the end of the workshop participants will have: · Shared experiences and highlighted current behaviors that challenge them in their practice · Explored the coach approach and determined what key aspects might help them with the delivery of services · Practiced coaching skills required to address challenges that impede successful service delivery · Increased confidence, self accountability and enthusiasm for a coach approach Multidisciplinary Assessment and Management of Auditory Processing Disorders James W. Hall III, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL; Diane P. Wertz, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL There is unprecedented interest in auditory processing disorders (APD) among audiologists, speech pathologists, and parents of children who are academic underachievers. Differential diagnosis of APD is challenging for many audiologists. Assessment procedures and protocols extend far beyond the audiogram, and treatment strategies are highly varied and must be closely coordinated with other professionals. Furthermore, APD must be differentiated from among a variety of co-existing disorders, such as dyslexia, language impairment, and ADHD. This exciting new workshop will provide the clinical audiologist or speech pathologist with a practical and logical multidisciplinary approach for assessment and management of this substantial and under-served population that’s based on a foundation of basic and applied science, and used daily by the University of Florida Department of Communicative Disorders and Multidisciplinary Diagnostic and Treatment Program. The session will provide the clinician with practical take-home messages. 58 Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 CASLPA 2006 Abstracts Audiology Workshops Selection and Verification of Modern Hearing Aids Gustav Mueller, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN Dr. Mueller will discuss the following: A. Features that are desired and/or required in modern hearing aids B. Formulating fitting goals C. Verifying fitting goals D. Verifying hearing aid features E. Troubleshooting patient problems Beyond the Basics: Exploring the Auditory System Barbara Reynolds, Green Valley, ON Temporal Processing and Spatial Acoustics: The auditory system is at least one order of magnitude faster in processing information than the visual system. Temporal auditory processing problems do affect how we hear and understand speech as well as other aspects including interhemispheric transfer and spatial processing. This presentation will include information on temporal processing and its impact on auditory perception developmentally, through the aging process and as a result of auditory deprivation. The auditory system does have a “what” and “where” pathway. This presentation will give information on the varying aspects of human auditory spatial perception, which goes beyond simple right/left sound localization. Included will be an overview of the neurophysiology and the consequences of the loss of spatial acoustics, but in perceptual terms and in its relationship to hearing aid amplification and counseling needs. Feedforward/Feedback Systems: Cortical Processing Systems Involved in Plasticity and Implications to Deprivation/Rehabilitation - Often when we think of the auditory system, we tend to limit ourselves to the ascending pathways. Different areas of the brain that are not often considered can impact the auditory system as far down as the cochlear nucleus and the cochlea. This presentation will include the concepts of feedforward (ascending) and feedback (descending) systems that can influence cortical plasticity, learning and rehabilitation/ deprivation. Understanding the complete auditory system can help us to communicate with patients and other medical professionals about the importance of the auditory system beyond the cochlea. Taking a History: Why Things Matter in a Patient’s History - Taking a patient’s history may seem straightforward, but being thorough involves much more than asking the standard questions of noise and listening difficulties. This presentation will include other questions regarding past history and health issues that can impact perception and can affect the adjustments or settings of hearing aids. Also included in this presentation will be counseling considerations in getting patients to understand the importance of early amplification and the realistic rehabilitation time course and outlook based on their individual history. Alzheimer’s Update: This presentation will continue the information supplied previously on the effects of Alzheimer’s disease on the auditory system and related systems. Included will be the effects on the neurophysiology and the perceptual changes one can expect in a patient with Alzheimer’s disease. Amplification and counseling issues will also be addressed. ASSR and Tinnitus James W. Hall III, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL The Role of the Auditory Steady State Response (ASSR) in Audiology Today This session will begin with a historical perspective on the ASSR followed by a review of principles and procedures for ASSR measurement. The role of the ASSR in the pediatric test battery will be defined with an emphasis on the use of ASSR in estimating auditory thresholds in infants and young children. Clinical advantages and disadvantages of ASSR in pediatric diagnostic assessment will be illustrated with original case reports. The session will also include discussion of other diagnostic clinical applications of the ASSR technique in pediatric and adult populations. Audiologic Assessment and Management of Tinnitus Tinnitus or hyperacusis affects millions of persons in the United States, Canada, and other developed countries. Within recent years, we have witnessed unprecedented research and clinical interest in tinnitus and hyperacusis. This instructional course will emphasize principles, protocols, and current practices important in the audiologic assessment and management of tinnitus, and will provide the practicing audiologist or otolaryngologist with a clinically feasible approach to caring effectively for this challenging and underserved patient population. The presentation will include a review of the basic mechanisms of tinnitus/hyperacusis, a detailed test battery for diagnostic tinnitus assessment, and a review of effective tinnitus treatment options. Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 59 CASLPA 2006 Abstracts Speech-Language Pathology Workshops Tracheostomy and Ventilator Dependency Management of Breathing, Speaking and Swallowing in Adults Donna Tippett, Departments of Otolaryngology – Head and Neck Surgery, and Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MA This course will be presented at an intermediate-advanced level, primarily for speech-language pathologists. Topic areas include clinical controversies, evidence-based practice, quality of life, and ethical considerations. There will also be an interactive portion to present and discuss challenging cases. Addressing Social Communication in Children and Adolescents with High Functioning Autism and Asperger’s Syndrome Emily Rubin, Director, Communication Crossroads, Carmel, CA The session will begin with a review of the core social and communicative challenges faced by children and adolescents with High Functioning Autism and Asperger’s Syndrome as well as our current understanding of the learning style differences that differentiate these disorders. Next, we will explore the essential components of a comprehensive educational program in order to establish guidelines for prioritizing educational and therapeutic objectives. Guidelines for determining appropriate learning supports and educational accommodations will also be provided. An emphasis will be placed on learning supports designed to address social communication and emotional regulation (e.g., video replay, friendship maps, and feelings books). Esophageal Phase Deglutition and Its Disorders: What Speech Pathologists Need to Know Caryn Easterling, University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee, Pewaskee, WI This half-day session by Caryn S. Easterling, Ph.D. will include the anatomy and physiology of the esophageal phase of deglutition in adults. A review of research in the area of esophageal physiology and pathophysiology will be presented with clinical interpretation and implications. Clinical instrumentation used for diagnosis and management of esophageal phase disorders will be discussed. The rationale, development, clinical application, and multidisciplinary team implementation of the Shaker Exercise, an exercise for deglutitive upper esophageal sphincter opening, will be discussed. Read with Me Shari Robertson, Phoenix Enterprises, Inc., Indiana, PA This workshop will provide background regarding the relationship between language and literacy (both deal with form, content, and use) and will demonstrate six interactive strategies that can be used to boost language and literacy across age groups, disability areas, and even taught to parents. Extensions on how to use interactive reading with older children and to boost formal reading skills can also be included Book lists, parent handouts, hands-on practice, etc., will be provided. Cleft Lip/Palate and Velopharyngeal Dysfunction: Effects on Speech and Resonance Ann Kummer, Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, OH This seminar will cover basic oral-pharyngeal anatomy, types of clefts, velopharyngeal dysfunction and resonance disorders. High tech, low tech and no-tech evaluation techniques will be described. Treatment options will be discussed, including surgery, prosthetic devices, and speech therapy. Specific therapy techniques will be demonstrated. Head and Neck Cancer/Oncology: An Update for S-LPs Candace Myers, CancerCare Manitoba, Winnipeg, MB This presentation will provide an overview of current management of patients with head and neck cancer, including treatment options, assessment and management of dysphagia and symptom issues, psychosocial care, and transition to end-of-life care. A review of functional outcomes from the current literature and at CancerCare Manitoba will be presented. References, resources, and direction for continuing education will be provided. Making Connections in the Classroom Shari Robertson, Phoenix Enterprises, Inc., Indiana, PA This workshop is geared toward intervention with school-aged students. It targets 3 major goals – increasing cognitive skills, increasing pragmatic skills and building linguistic and metalinguistic skills with an emphasis on written language skills (Narrative and Expository). The workshop will provide numerous examples and lesson plans. 60 Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 CASLPA 2006 Abstracts Complementary and Alternative Treatment of Aphasia Kristine Lundgren, Harold Goodglass Aphasia Research Center, Boston, MA The field of Complementary and Alternative Medicine (CAM) is a fast-growing component of many medical and rehabilitation programs and has been integrated into internal medicine, oncology, psychology, physical therapy and occupational therapy. This workshop will provide background information about the field, will describe preliminary research to support the use of some nontraditional approaches (relaxation, animal-assisted therapy, MindfulnessBased Stress Reduction, acupuncture, Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation), and will discuss ways in which some of these therapies can be incorporated into traditional speech and language practices. Case studies with communicatively impaired adults and adolescents will be presented. This course will be presented at an intermediate level. Training Preschool Teachers to Promote Language and Literacy Development Janice Greenberg, The Hanen Centre, Toronto, ON Learn about a research-validated approach for training early childhood teachers to promote children’s social, language and literacy development in naturalistic settings. Through a unique combination of group training sessions and individualized videotaping and feedback sessions, teachers learn how to use play and daily activities to create inclusive interactive language-learning environments. The S-LP’s Role in the Treatment of Selective Mutism and Other Psychogenic Disorders Suzanne Hungerford, Plattsburgh State University of New York, Plattsburgh, NY Although psychogenic communication disorders are not common, speech-language pathologists (S-LPs) do encounter individuals with these disorders, and when they do, they often feel ill-prepared to meet their needs. The goal of this session will be to familiarize S-LPs with some of the disorders and to offer an introduction to treatment. Audiology Contributed Papers Can You Provide What Consumers Want/Need? Michel David, Janice McNamara, Canadian Hard of Hearing Association, Ottawa, ON This workshop will outline the findings of the of 2005 Hearing Awareness Survey of Hearing Loss. It will suggest some cause and effect analysis to understand some of the issues expressed by consumers. The responses are instructive as they help identify where problems lie and what issues need to be addressed. Speech-Language Pathology Contributed Papers Multiple System Atrophy: Multiple Responsibilities for Speech-Language Pathologists Richard J. Welland, Brock University, St. Catharines, ON Multiple system atrophy is a term that encompasses a group of adult-onset neurodegenerative conditions, such as Shy-Drager Syndrome. Patients with multiple system atrophy typically develop a triad of signs and symptoms: dysarthria; dysphagia; and dementia. The combination of these three disorders represents a particular challenge for the speech-language pathologist. Dysphagia Care Teams for Stroke Survivors in the LTC Setting Rosemary Martino, Becky French, Shelley Sharp, Lisa Durkin, UHN, Toronto Western Hospital, Toronto, ON Dysphagia care teams will be established for stroke survivors in three long-term care (LTC) facilities within the Toronto West Regional Stroke Network. Barriers to implementation, and staff knowledge and satisfaction will be assessed. Establishing dysphagia care teams will aid appropriate identification of stroke survivors requiring consultation by an S-LP. Outcome Measurement in Aphasia: A Framework for “Counting What Counts” Aura Kagan, Jennifer Hicks, Elyse Shumway, The Aphasia Institute, Toronto, ON; Nina Simmons-Mackie, Southeastern Louisiana University and The Aphasia Institute, Abita Springs, LA; Maria Huijbregts, Baycrest Centre for Geriatric Care, Toronto, ON; Sara McEwen, The Aphasia Institute, Hawkestone, ON This presentation will identify gaps in outcome measurement related to real-life participation and living with aphasia. The authors will present a user-friendly and accessible framework for clinical aphasiologists that captures key concepts in current thinking within and beyond our field and apply this specifically to measuring real-life outcomes of aphasia interventions. Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 61 CASLPA 2006 Abstracts Aphasia in Residential Care Jennifer Sweeney, Private Practice, Vancouver, BC In recent years speech-language pathology practice has evolved to incorporate the principles of client-centred and family-centred care. We have knowledge and tools that encourage increased independence and quality of life. This session will focus on the challenges of system-wide support for people with aphasia when admitted to residential care. Communication Needs of Nonspeaking Adults in Acute Care: Survey Results Colleen A. Braun-Janzen, Leslie Mennell, Health Sciences Centre, Winnipeg, MB This study investigated practice patterns, and opinions of best practice standards regarding management of nonspeaking adult patients in acute care. Survey results suggest that S-LP staffing levels in many acute care facilities are insufficient to adequately respond to communication needs and nurses are frequently facilitating hands-on communication intervention. Attitudes and Perceptions of Adults with Severe Acquired Communication Disorders Colleen A. Braun-Janzen, Deer Lodge Centre, Winnipeg, MB Fourteen adults with acquired communication disorders who use AAC were interviewed. Respondents reflected on benefits and frustrations of using AAC, transition from being a verbal communicator to using AAC, and suggestions for verbal communicators in interfacing with people who use AAC. Assessment of Higher Level Cognitive-Communication Deficits Sheila MacDonald, Sheila MacDonald & Associates, Guelph, ON Assessment of subtle cognitive-communication deficits following acquired brain injuries can pose a significant clinical challenge for speech-language pathologists. The Functional Assessment of Verbal Reasoning and Executive Strategies (FAVRES) is a standardized test that was designed to meet this challenge. This presentation will discuss issues of assessment of cognitive-communication deficits and present the results of a normative study of the FAVRES. Outcome Measures for Children with ASD Following Parent Training Tara Davies, Luigi Girolametto, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON; Fern Sussman, Elaine Weitzman, The Hanen Centre, Toronto, ON This presentation will summarize outcomes following participation in More Than Words, a parent program for children with autism spectrum disorder. Following the program, all 12 children demonstrated increases in the length of joint attention episodes. The parents’ outcomes, examined using the Joy and Fun Scale (JAFA; McConachie & Currill, 2002) revealed improvements in the total score of JAFA from pretest to posttest. An Introduction to Relationship Development Intervention™ for Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorders Stephannie R. Motuz, Rehabilitation Centre for Children, Winnipeg, MB This presentation will define Relationship Development Intervention ™ reviewing the service delivery model and the consultant certification process. A brief overview of the core deficits targeted for remediation and the intervention techniques will precede two case studies. A description of the local ongoing efficacy study of RDI techniques will also be offered to the attendee. Exploring the Ethical Dimension of Paediatric Speech-Language Pathology Eleanor Stewart, University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB This study developed a substantive account of ethical practice for speech-language pathologists working with paediatric populations. Using grounded theory methodology, the research explored the perspectives of clinicians active in clinical practice. The resulting model identified the central moral aim and defined ethical actions and the characteristics of the ethical clinician. Meeting the Clinical Education Challenge! Susan J. Wagner, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON; Lu-Anne McFarlane, University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB; Speakers TBD from Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS, McGill University, Montreal, QC, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, University of Laval, Laval, QC, University of Montreal, Montreal, QC, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, ON and University of Western Ontario, London, ON Research and experience indicate that clinicians receive minimal formal preparation in providing best practice clinical education to students. The objectives of this panel session of academic coordinators of clinical education will be to examine the clinical education process and share successful strategies among participants to meet the clinical education challenge. 62 Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 CASLPA 2006 Abstracts Something Tells Me We’re Not in the Therapy Room Anymore John Serkiz, New Brunswick Department of Health and Wellness, Fredericton, NB; Michele Lonergan, Miramichi Regional Health Authority, Miramichi, NB New Brunswick has implemented Parle-moi/Talk With Me Services, which support communication development in all preschool children. This unique service is expected to have a positive impact on school readiness skills across the province through promotion, prevention, and community capacity building. Using the ENNI for Narrative Assessment or Norm Development Phyllis Schneider, Denyse Hayward, University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB; Rita V. Dube, Toronto District School Board, Toronto, ON In this seminar, we will present an instrument for collecting information about storytelling skills from children aged 4-9, the Edmonton Narrative Norms Instrument (ENNI). We will describe how the ENNI is used to assess children’s storytelling; hands-on practice will be provided. We will also discuss how to develop local norms. Poster Sessions SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Communication Skills in Creutzfeldt-Jacob Disease: A Case Vignette Gopee Krishnan, Raj Shekar, Manipal Academy of Higher Education, Manipal, Karnataka, India Creutzfeldt-Jacob Disease (CJD) is an extremely rare, rapidly progressing, fatal condition with a unique clinical phenotype. Communication skills and various other cognitive and motor skills are often compromised. This case report will attempt to highlight the clinical characteristics, pathophysiology, findings of neurological and cognitivelinguistic evaluations, and management strategies in CJD. FLUENCY Subjective Severity Ratings of the Effect of SpeechEasy® on Stuttering Marie S. Mossman, Cape Breton Family Place Resource Centre, Sydney, NS; Tiffany M. Steeves, Berry Mills, NB; Joy Armson, Michael Kiefte, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS Participants rated the stuttering severity of oral reading samples made before and after persons-who-stutter wore a SpeechEasy® device. Stuttering severity decreased by 3.31 points on a 9-point scale for the device compared to the no-device condition. Except in two cases, a linear relationship between severity ratings and stuttering frequencies was found. DYSPHAGIA The Need for Dysphagia Care Matrices in Acute Care Settings Genefer Behamdouni, St. Jospeh’s Health Centre, Toronto, ON In the past, case prioritization has been based on a variety of clinical, emotional, and time pressure factors. This presentation will discuss a standardized approach to case load management, which supports clinical decision making and reduces patient risk, and the development of an Adult Care Matrix. Feeding and Swallowing Management: Manitoba Guidelines and Winnipeg Policy Shelley Irvine Day, Angela Forrest Kenning, Kelly Tye Vallis, Monique Piatt, Deer Lodge Centre, Winnipeg, MB In 2001, Manitoba Health distributed the Manual for Feeding and Swallowing Management in Long-Term Care (LTC) Facilities to all personal care homes in Manitoba. The Winnipeg Regional Health Authority (WRHA) coined these guidelines into policy in 2004. This poster will outline the breadth of the statements contained in both documents, the implications on speech-language pathology and the implementation process in the WRHA. Effortful Versus Non-Effortful Swallowing: Investigating Laryngeal Movement Patterns Veronique L. Philbin, Rebecca C. Smith, Syed Salman Qadri, Ruth E. Martin, University of Western Ontario, London, ON We sought to determine whether effortful and non-effortful swallowing could be differentiated through the use of a laryngeal movement transducer in healthy subjects. Results indicated that effortful and non-effortful swallowing produce significantly different patterns of laryngeal movement. Thus, techniques for recording and displaying laryngeal movement may prove to be effective biofeedback approaches for dysphagic patients. Enhancing Dysphagia Rehabilitation: Is There a Role for the ICF? Julie A. Theurer, University of Western Ontario, London, ON This presentation will review traditional approaches to dysphagia rehabilitation, and examine issues arising in the recent dysphagia literature that highlight the need for innovative new clinical and research practices. The role of the International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health (ICF) in enhancing dysphagia rehabilitation will be explored. Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 63 CASLPA 2006 Abstracts CHILD LANGUAGE Efficacy of Phonological Awareness Intervention for Children with Down Syndrome Patricia L. Cleave, Elizabeth Kay-Raining Bird, Katie MacIsaac, Melissa Armstrong, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS; Derrick C. Bourassa, Acadia University, Wolfville, NS This poster will describe the results of a training program designed to facilitate phonological awareness skills in children with Down syndrome. Eight children were involved in the 22-week program. The response to the intervention was variable such that 4 of 8 children showed gains. Variables predicting success will be discussed. “Let’s Read and Talk”: A Community Preschool Language and Literacy Program Janet P. Simpson, Winnipeg Regional Health Authority, Winnipeg, MB This presentation will describe a community-based health promotion program designed to enhance caregiver skills to facilitate language and literacy development in preschool children. Using storybook reading, “best practice” strategies were presented, facilitated and coached. Evaluation of pre- and post-program videotapes has shown substantive and statistically significant positive changes. Computer-Assisted Treatment: Effects and Utilization in Speech-Language Pathology Karla N. Washington, Genese A. Warr-Leeper, University of Western Ontario, London, ON The pre-to-post outcomes of two computer-assisted treatment (C-AT) programs for preschoolers were examined. One program targeted phonology and the other language. The effects of the phonology program included increased accuracy and intelligibility and the effects of the language program were increased accuracy and variety of sentence structures, length, and complexity. Recast Density and Acquisition of Irregular Past Tense Verbs Kerry E. Proctor-Williams, East Tennessee State University, Johnson City, TN; Marc E. Fey, University of Kansas Medical Center, Kansas City, KS At conversational recast rates 13 children with typical language produced irregular past verbs more accurately than 13 children with SLI. At intervention recast rates verb accuracy did not improve in the SLI group and the TL group’s performance declined. The children’s metalinguistic productions suggest the groups used recast information differently. ADULT SERVICES Analyse de la syntaxe chez les aphasiques de Broca et de Wernicke Fouzia Badaoui, CRSTDLA, Alger This paper presents the results of a research on two aphasics to examine their use of syntax based on the neokhalilien linguistic model. Effect of Language Therapy in Non-Fluent Primary Progressive Aphasia (NFPPA) Annie Delyfer, Fanny Singer, CRIR, Jewish Rehabilitation Hospital, Laval, QC; Eva Kehayia, McGill University and CRIR, Montreal, QC; Nancy Azevedo, McGill University, Montreal, QC This poster presentation will address the usefulness of traditional language therapy with NFPPA patients. A review of the literature as well as the test results of one NFPPA patient taken at three points in time indicate that language therapy can improve some language functions for a certain period of time. Crossed Aphasia and Singing Ability Patrick Coppens, Plattsburgh State University of New York, Plattsburgh, NY; Sylvie Hébert, Université de Montréal, Montreal, QC; Lise Gagnon, Université de Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, QC Crossed aphasia is caused by a lesion in the right hemisphere in a right-hander. Does this unusual language lateralization pattern influence the neurological organization of right hemisphere skills, such as singing? Illustrated by a clinical case, this poster presentation will attempt to answer this question and to extrapolate to aphasia rehabilitation issues. Development of an Interdisciplinary Low Tolerance Long Duration (LTLD) Stroke Rehabilitation Program Lauren Murphy, Fabian Krupski, St. Joseph’s Health Centre, Toronto, ON Rehabilitation needs of stroke survivors are not homogenous. This presentation will examine the development of an interdisciplinary low-tolerance, long duration (LTLD) stroke rehab program in a long-term care facility, designed specifically for individuals who because of their age or severity of their stroke, are in need of more specialized services. 64 Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 CASLPA 2006 Abstracts Role of Phonology in Word Identification: A Comparison of College Readers Randeep K. Sidhu, University of North Dakota, Winnipeg, MB; Charlene Chamberlain, University of North Dakota, Grand Forks, ND Undergraduate students (11 good, 11 average, and 9 poor readers) completed a reading test, a lexical decision and a naming task with pseudohomophone stimuli to examine the role of phonology in word recognition as a function of reading skills. Results showed no interaction. Clinical implications will be discussed. CLINICAL PRACTICE Competency-Based Intervention: What Would It Look Like? Carolyn Cronk, Julie Fortier-Blanc, Louise Duchesne, Université de Montréal, Montreal, QC This presentation will explore the implications of approaching speech and language intervention as a process of establishing a series of competencies or functional capabilities constructed from a combination of accumulated knowledge or awareness, basic abilities, and practiced skills. It will be illustrated by several clinical examples. S-LP Practice Forges Ahead with Personal Digital Assistants Alexa Okrainec, Brandon University, Brandon, MB The Personal Digital Assistant (PDA), a powerful handheld computer, is an innovation that can advance the delivery of speech-language pathology services. This presentation will feature the usefulness of the PDA in clinical practice, going beyond the popularized date and address book functions. Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 65 Congrès de l’ACOA 2006 - Abrégés C o ngrès de l’ACOA 2006 Winnip anit o ba innipee g , M Manit anito du 3 au 6 mai 2006 Ateliers pré-congrès Trachéotomie et gestion de la respiration par ventilation assistée, verbalisation et déglutition chez les adultes Donna Tippett, Départements d’oto-rhino-laryngologie – chirurgie de la tête et du cou, de médecine physique et de réadaptation, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore (Maryland) La gestion des troubles de verbalisation et de déglutition chez les adultes ayant subi une trachéotomie et respirant par ventilation assistée pose tout un défi. Cet atelier présentera de l’information de niveau de base et intermédiaire pour préparer les orthophonistes à évaluer et à traiter des adultes ayant des troubles de verbalisation et de déglutition. Le contenu de l’atelier intéressera aussi les ergothérapeutes, les physiothérapeutes, les infirmières et les inhalothérapeutes. Le modèle SCERTS : une approche pédagogique complète et multidisciplinaire pour les enfants aux prises avec les troubles du spectre autistique Emily Rubin, Director, Communication Crossroads, Carmel (Californie) Cet atelier définira le modèle SCERTS, une approche pédagogique complète et multidisciplinaire conçue pour augmenter les capacités socio-émotionnelles et de communication des enfants aux prises avec les troubles du spectre autistique et pour offrir un soutien aux familles et aux professionnels. L’acronyme SCERTS fait référence à trois grandes dimensions du modèle. Les objectifs de développement en communication sociale (social communication – SC) et la régulation des émotions (emotional regulation – ER) sont atteints grâce à des soutiens au traitement interactif (transactional supports – TS) (p. ex. : adaptation du style interpersonnel, accommodement de l’environnement, soutiens visuels, etc.) à chaque instant du quotidien de l’enfant et chez les partenaires sociaux afin de faciliter la compétence dans les domaines ciblés des contextes fonctionnels et significatifs (p. ex. : à domicile, à l’école et dans la collectivité). L’atelier débutera par un survol de la documentation actuelle sur l’efficacité des traitements et sur les lignes directrices de traitement pour la programmation pédagogique mises de l’avant par la National Academy of Science aux États-Unis. Cette présentation sera suivie par des lignes directrices pratiques pour mettre en œuvre ce modèle, ce qui sera illustré par des études de cas sur vidéo. Ateliers en orthophonie et audiologie Donner libre cours au potentiel des particuliers et des services Jim McLaren, Winnipeg (Man.) Les intervenants avant-gardistes comprennent que les relations constituent la véritable « monnaie » actuelle et future du monde du travail et que les techniques d’encadrement peuvent servir à nouer des relations. L’encadrement fonctionne sur le principe que le client est celui qui possède la clé des réponses à toutes les situations. L’encadrement sert à l’aider à trouver ces solutions. Alison Hendron, directrice de la formation pour la programme Certified Executive Coaching à la Royal Roads University, décrit l’encadrement en ces termes : « Prenons l’exemple de quelqu’un qui veut apprendre à faire de la bicyclette : un consultant lui explique les différentes bicyclettes et lui recommande la meilleure pour sa situation; un thérapeute l’aide à surmonter sa peur de tomber; et la personne chargée de l’encadrement court à côté d’elle pour s’assurer qu’elle ne tombera pas avant de pouvoir pédaler seule ». Cette séance plénière vise à partager l’état des connaissances sur la puissance de l’encadrement et sur toutes les possibilités qu’il offre pour l’exercice clinique. Ateliers en audiologie J’avais une emprise sur ma vie, mais je ne l’ai plus Shari Robertson, Phoenix Enterprises, Indiana (Pennsylvanie) Cette séance offrira des stratégies éprouvées pour gérer le temps et le stress. Elle s’adresse aux professionnels dans le domaine des troubles de la communication. Cette séance se déroulera à un rythme accéléré et fera souvent appel à la participation de l’auditoire. Il y aura beaucoup de rires. 66 Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 Congrès de l’ACOA 2006 - Abrégés Couverture par l’assurance/une tierce partie – Démystifier le monde des assurances et des tiers payants Katherine Cheney, Stewart McIntosh, Anciens combattants Canada; Tania Stote, Croix bleue Medavie; Jocelyne Lavergne-Robenhymer, Direction générale de la santé des Premières nations et des Inuits, Services de santé non assurés (SSNA); Michael O’Brien, Agence du revenu du Canada : Crédit d’impôt pour personnes handicapées Cette séance vous offrira l’occasion de découvrir les politiques et les mécanismes de financement du ministère canadien des Anciens combattants, de la Commission des accidents du travail, des Services de santé non assurés, de la Croix bleue et d’autres tiers payants et compagnies d’assurance. Vous aurez l’occasion de poser des questions particulières à chacun des conférenciers. Soyons critique!: que faut-il chercher dans les rapports de recherche que nous lisons Lisa Lix, Université du Manitoba, Winnipeg (Man.) L’expression « prise de décisions fondée sur des résultats scientifiques » est presque devenue un cliché. Mais dans les faits, comment est-il possible d’utiliser effectivement des résultats scientifiques pour prendre des décisions au travail? Cette séance mettra l’accent sur trois principaux éléments qui doivent ressortir d’un examen critique d’une étude : (1) le message : quelle sont les principales conclusions? (2) la méthode : les conclusions sont-elles justifiées par rapport aux données et à l’analyse? (3) le portrait d’ensemble : les résultats sont-ils pertinents pour les clients qui viennent me consulter? Conception universelle : une éducation sensée pour tous les élèves Karen Priestley, Winnipeg (Man.) La conception universelle est un nouveau cadre intéressant pour instaurer de nouvelles façons de répondre aux besoins de notre population étudiante variée. Il s’agit d’un mouvement vers l’actualisation de l’inclusion dans le sens le plus pur de l’expression puisque ce cadre voit les élèves du point de vue de leurs besoins et n’adhère pas à la catégorisation des incapacités. La conception universelle met l’accent sur l’accès de tous les élèves au programme. Elle sous-entend que l’on cesse d’adapter les instructions, les programmes et les évaluations afin de tenir compte des besoins variés ou exceptionnels des élèves à mesure qu’ils se manifestent (inclusion) pour plutôt faire preuve de clairvoyance en prévoyant et en planifiant des moyens de répondre à tous les besoins de manière unilatérale (conception universelle). Cette méthode d’enseignement n’est pas radicalement nouvelle, mais elle élargit plutôt nombre de nos façons de faire actuelles et mise sur ce que les personnes qui œuvrent dans le domaine de l’enseignement savent déjà à partir de la recherche et de la pratique au sujet des bonnes méthodes d’enseignement et de la création de milieux accessibles. La conception universelle est englobante et fait appel à tous les membres de la communauté d’apprentissage. Les bases théoriques de la conception universelle sont issus du domaine de l’architecture et sont utilisées en éducation depuis environ 1990. La conception universelle est particulièrement associée avec le champ de l’éducation spéciale. Ce domaine se sert de la conception universelle pour faire en sorte que le programme général est accessible à tous les élèves peu importe les besoins en matière d’apprentissage. La mise en œuvre de la conception universelle fait appel à la restructuration, à la créativité et à la collaboration. Une méthode d’entraînement Jim McLaren, Winnipeg (Man.) Cette séance présentera une approche dynamique et fondée sur l’expérience pour l’apprentissage. La journée est conçue pour offrir la souplesse maximale afin de traiter les « points de douleur » et de répondre aux besoins ainsi qu’aux intérêts de chaque participant. Elle met l’accent sur la discussion de groupe et mise sur la sagesse et l’expérience collective des participants. Les activités portent sur des techniques d’apprentissage, telles que les études de cas, les exercices pratiques, les jeux de rôles, les démonstrations, la prise de conscience de soi et les travaux en équipe. À la fin de la séance, les participants auront : · partagé leur expérience et fait ressortir les comportements qui leur posent des défis dans leur pratique · mis à l’essai la méthode d’entraînement et déterminé les aspects clés qui peuvent les aider à assurer la prestation de leurs services · mis à l’essai des techniques d’entraînement nécessaires pour relever les défis qui nuisent à la prestation des services · rehaussé la confiance en leurs moyens, leur responsabilisation à leur égard et leur niveau d’enthousiasme vis-à-vis de la méthode d’entraînement Évaluation multidisciplinaire et gestion des troubles de traitement des informations auditives James W. Hall III, University of Florida, Gainesville (Floride); Diane P. Wertz, University of Florida, Gainesville, (Floride) Les troubles de traitement des informations auditives suscitent un intérêt sans précédent chez les audiologistes, les orthophonistes et les parents d’enfants qui ne réussissent pas bien à l’école. Le diagnostic différentiel de ces troubles pose cependant des défis pour les audiologistes. Les méthodes et les protocoles d’évaluation vont largement au-delà de l’audiogramme et les stratégies de traitement sont très variées et doivent être étroitement coordonnées Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 67 Congrès de l’ACOA 2006 - Abrégés avec d’autres professionnels. De plus, il faut distinguer ces troubles d’une série d’autres troubles concomitants, tels que la dyslexie, les troubles du langage et l’hyperactivité avec déficit de l’attention. Ce nouvel atelier intéressant fournira à l’audiologiste et à l’orthophoniste en milieu clinique une approche multidisciplinaire pratique et logique pour évaluer et gérer cette population substantielle et mal desservie. Fondée sur des sciences fondamentales et appliquées, cette approche est utilisée quotidiennement au département des troubles de la communication, programme de diagnostic et de traitement multidisciplinaire, de l’University of Florida. Cette séance offrira au clinicien des messages pratiques qui lui serviront de retour chez lui. Ateliers en audiologie Sélection et vérification des appareils auditifs modernes Gustav Mueller, Vanderbilt University, Nashville (Tennessee) Le Dr Mueller abordera les points suivants : A. Caractéristiques souhaitables ou nécessaires dans un appareil moderne B. Établissement des objectifs d’appareillage C. Vérification des objectifs d’appareillage D. Vérification des caractéristiques de l’appareil auditif E. Diagnostic des difficultés éprouvées par le patient Au-delà de l’essentiel : explorer le système auditif Barbara Reynolds, Green Valley (Ont.) Traitement temporel et acoustique spatiale : Le système auditif traite l’information au moins une fois plus rapidement que le système visuel. Les troubles de traitement auditif temporel ont une incidence sur ce que nous entendons et ce que nous comprenons ainsi que sur d’autres aspects, y compris le transfert interhémisphérique et le traitement spatial. Cette séance présentera de l’information sur le traitement temporel et son incidence sur le développement de la perception auditive, par le biais du vieillissement et à la suite d’une carence auditive. Le système auditif possède une voie du « quoi » et du « où ». Cette séance présentera divers aspects de la perception spatiale auditive chez les humains, qui depasse simplement la localisation à gauche ou à droite d’un son. Elle fera un survol de la neurophysiologie et des conséquences de la perte de l’acoustique spatiale, mais du point de vue de la perception et de sa relation avec les appareils auditifs et les besoins de counselling. Systèmes de stimulation ascendante et descendante : systèmes de traitement du cortex qui influent sur la plasticité et incidences sur la carence/réadaptation auditive – Lorsque nous pensons au système auditif, nous nous limitons souvent aux voies ascendantes. Divers domaines du cerveau qui ne sont pas souvent pris en considération peuvent pourtant avoir une incidence sur le système auditif, aussi loin que le noyau cochléaire et la cochlée. Cette séance porte sur les concepts de systèmes de stimulation ascendante (feedforward) et de stimulation descendante (feedback) qui peuvent influencer la plasticité corticale, l’apprentissage et la réadaptation/ carence auditive. Le fait de comprendre le fonctionnement complet du système auditif peut nous aider à faire comprendre aux patients et aux autres professionnels de la santé l’importance du système auditif au-delà de la cochlée. Antécédents médicaux : pourquoi est-ce important – Il peut sembler tout simple de noter les antécédents médicaux d’un patient, mais il ne faut pas se limiter à poser les questions habituelles sur les bruits entendus ou les difficultés d’écoute. Cette séance abordera d’autres questions concernant les antécédents et la santé qui peuvent avoir une incidence sur la perception et influer sur l’ajustement ou le réglage des appareils auditifs. De plus, elle traitera aussi du counselling pour faire comprendre aux patients l’importance de l’amplification précoce, l’échéancier réaliste de la réadaptation ainsi que les résultats individuels à escompter. Mise à jour sur l’Alzheimer : Cette séance se veut la suite d’une autre précédente sur les effets de l’Alzheimer sur le système auditif et les systèmes connexes. Elle traitera des effets sur la neurophysiologie et les changements perceptuels auxquels on peut s’attendre chez un patient souffrant de la maladie d’Alzheimer. Les questions liées à l’amplification et au counselling seront aussi abordées. ASSR et acouphène James W. Hall III, University of Florida, Gainesville (Floride) Rôle des réponses auditives à l’état stable en audiologie de nos jours Cette séance débute par un survol historique des réponses auditives à l’état stable (RAÉS) suivi par un examen des principes et des procédures de mesure de ces réponses. Le rôle des RAÉS dans la batterie de tests en pédiatrie sera défini en mettant l’accent sur l’utilisation de ces réponses pour évaluer le seuil d’audition des enfants en bas âge. Les avantages et les inconvénients en milieu clinique des RAÉS pour l’évaluation des enfants seront illustrés grâce à des exposés de cas originaux. La séance comprendra aussi une discussion sur d’autres applications cliniques des RAÉS chez les enfants et les adultes. 68 Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 Congrès de l’ACOA 2006 - Abrégés Évaluation audiologique et gestion de l’acouphène L’acouphène ou l’hyperacousie affecte des millions de personnes aux États-Unis, au Canada et dans d’autres pays développés. Au cours des dernières années, un nombre de recherches sans précédent ont été menées dans ce domaine, qui a aussi suscité beaucoup d’intérêt en milieu clinique. Ce cours d’instruction mettra l’accent sur les principes, les protocoles et les pratiques actuels qui importent pour l’évaluation audiologique et la gestion de l’acouphène. Il fournira aussi aux audiologistes et aux oto-rhino-laryngologistes en exercice une méthode réaliste en milieu clinique pour traiter efficacement cette population complexe et mal desservie. La présentation comprendra un survol des mécanismes de base de l’acouphène ou hyperacousie, une batterie de test détaillée pour établir un diagnostic et un examen des options de traitement. Ateliers en orthophonie Trachéotomie et gestion de la respiration par ventilation assistée, verbalisation et déglutition chez les adultes Donna Tippett, Départements d’oto-rhino-laryngologie – chirurgie de la tête et du cou, de médecine physique et de réadaptation, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore (Maryland) Ce cours présentera de l’information de niveau intermédiaire et avancé et s’adresse tout particulièrement aux orthophonistes. Il abordera les polémiques du monde clinique, l’exercice fondé sur les résultats cliniques et scientifiques, la qualité de vie et les dilemmes d’ordre éthique. Il comprend aussi une partie interactive où les participants pourront présenter des cas difficiles et en discuter. La communication sociale chez les enfants et les adolescents aux prises avec l’autisme de haut niveau et le syndrome d’Asperger Emily Rubin, Director, Communication Crossroads, Carmel (Californie) Cette séance débutera par un survol des principaux défis sociaux et troubles de communication des enfants et des adolescents aux prises avec l’autisme de haut niveau et le syndrome d’Asperger. Elle passera aussi en revue l’état actuel de nos connaissances sur les divers styles d’apprentissage qui distinguent ces troubles. Ensuite, elle explorera les composantes essentielles d’un programme d’éducation complet en vue d’établir des lignes directrices pour classer par ordre de priorité les objectifs d’éducation et de thérapie. Les lignes directrices pour déterminer les aides appropriées à l’apprentissage et les aménagements éducatifs nécessaires seront également abordées. La séance mettra l’accent sur les aides à l’apprentissage conçus pour améliorer la communication sociale et la régulation des émotions (p. ex. : reprise vidéo, cartes des amitiés et livrets des sentiments). Phase œsophagienne de la déglutition et ses troubles : ce que doit savoir tout orthophoniste Caryn Easterling, University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee, Pewaskee (Wisconsin) Cette séance d’une demi-journée animée par Caryn S. Easterling, Ph. D., portera sur l’anatomie et la physiologie de la phase œsophagienne de la déglutition chez les adultes. Elle présentera un survol de la recherche dans le domaine de la physiologie et de la pathophysiologie de l’œsophage, les interprétations cliniques et les incidences. Les instruments cliniques qui servent à poser un diagnostic et à gérer les troubles de la phase œsophagienne de la déglutition y seront également abordés. La séance présentera le raisonnement derrière l’exercice de Shaker, son élaboration, son application clinique et son utilisation par une équipe multidisciplinaire. Cet exercice vise à renforcer l’ouverture du sphincter supérieur de l’œsophage lors de la déglutition. Lis avec moi Shari Robertson, Phoenix Enterprises, Indiana (Pennsylvanie) Cette séance mettra en contexte la relation entre la langue et l’apprentissage de la lecture et de l’écriture (les deux font appel à la forme, au contenu et à l’utilisation) et démontrera six stratégies interactives pour favoriser ces deux aspects chez tous les groupes d’âge et pour toutes les formes de déficiences. Ces stratégies peuvent même être enseignées aux parents. Il est aussi possible d’élargir les stratégies pour inclure des façons d’utiliser la lecture interactive avec des enfants plus âgés et de rehausser les aptitudes à la lecture. Des listes de livres, de la documentation pour les parents, des exercices pratiques et autres ressources seront fournis. Fente labiale/palatine et dysfonctionnement vélopharyngé : effets sur la parole et la résonance Ann Kummer, Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati (Ohio) Cette séance présentera l’anatomie oro-pharyngée de base, les types de fente, le dysfonctionnement vélopharyngé et les troubles de résonance. Elle décrira les techniques d’évaluation de haute technologie et de base. Les options de traitement y seront abordées, y compris la chirurgie, les prothèses et l’orthophonie, et des techniques de thérapie précises y seront démontrées. Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 69 Congrès de l’ACOA 2006 - Abrégés Cancer/oncologie de la tête et du cou : des nouvelles pour les orthophonistes Candace Myers, CancerCare Manitoba, Winnipeg (Man.) Cette séance effectuera un survol de la gestion actuelle des patients atteints d’un cancer de la tête et du cou, y compris les options de traitement, l’évaluation et la gestion de la dysphagie et des symptômes, les soins psychosociaux et la transition vers les soins en fin de vie. Elle présentera aussi un examen des résultats fonctionnels contenus dans la documentation actuelle et obtenus par CancerCare Manitoba. Enfin, elle fournira des références, des ressources et une orientation pour la formation professionnelle continue. Établir des liens en salle de classe Shari Robertson, Phoenix Enterprises, Indiana (Pennsylvanie) Cet atelier porte sur l’intervention auprès des enfants d’âge scolaire. Il vise trois objectifs principaux : accroître les capacités cognitives, accroître les capacités pragmatiques et renforcer les capacités linguistiques et métalinguistiques, notamment des capacités langagières (narration et exposition). Cet atelier fournira de nombreux exemples et des plans de leçon. Traitement de l’aphasie par thérapie complémentaire et parallèle Kristine Lundgren, Harold Goodglass Aphasia Research Center, Boston (Massachusetts) Le champ de la médecine complémentaire et parallèle occupe une place en plein essor dans bien des programmes de médecine et de réadaptation. Il a été intégré dans la médecine interne, l’oncologie, la psychologie, la physiothérapie et l’ergothérapie. Cette séance mettra ce champ en contexte, décrira les recherches préliminaires qui sont favorables à l’utilisation d’approches non traditionnelles (relaxation, zoothérapie, pleine conscience du moment présent, acupuncture, stimulation magnétique crânienne), et expliquera des façons d’intégrer certaines de ces thérapies dans l’orthophonie traditionnelle. Des études de cas auprès d’adultes souffrant de troubles de la communication seront aussi présentées. La séance sera de niveau intermédiaire. Former les éducatrices de la petite enfance à favoriser l’acquisition du langage et l’apprentissage de la lecture et de l’écriture Janice Greenberg, The Hanen Centre, Toronto (Ont.) Cette séance présentera une démarche fondée sur la recherche pour former les éducatrices de la petite enfance à favoriser le développement social et linguistique ainsi que l’apprentissage de la lecture et de l’écriture dans des milieux naturels. Grâce à une combinaison de formation en groupe et à des séances individuelles de filmage et de rétroaction, les éducatrices apprennent comment utiliser le jeu et les activités quotidiennes pour créer des milieux inclusifs propices à l’apprentissage du langage. Rôle de l’orthophoniste dans le traitement du mutisme sélectif et d’autres troubles psychogènes Suzanne Hungerford, Plattsburgh State University of New York, Plattsburgh (New York) Bien que les troubles de communication psychogènes ne soient pas courants, les orthophonistes reçoivent en consultation des personnes qui en sont atteintes. Dans ces cas, ils se sentent souvent mal préparés pour répondre aux besoins de cette clientèle. Cette séance vise à mieux faire connaître certains de ces troubles aux orthophonistes et à aborder le traitement. Les présentations proposées en audiologie Pouvez-vous offrir ce que les consommateurs cherchent/veulent? Michel David, Janice McNamara, Association des malentendants canadiens, Ottawa (Ont.) Cette communication abordera les conclusions de l’enquête 2005 sur la sensibilité à la perte auditive. Elle présentera une analyse des causes et effets pour comprendre certaines des préoccupations formulées par les consommateurs. Les réponses à cette enquête sont révélatrices puisqu’elles mettent en lumière les problèmes et les enjeux dont il faut tenir compte. Les présentations proposées en orthophonie Syndrome de Shy-Drager: des responsabilités multiples pour les orthophonistes Richard J. Welland, Brock University, St. Catharines (Ont.) L’atrophie multisystématisée est une expression qui comprend un groupe de troubles neurodégénératifs chez l’adulte, notamment le syndrome de Shy-Drager. Les patients atteints de ce type d’atrophie présentent généralement trois signes et symptômes : dysarthrie, dysphagie et démence. La combinaison de ces trois troubles pose des défis particuliers pour les orthophonistes. Équipes de soins de longue durée en dysphagie pour les survivants d’un accident vasculaire cérébral Rosemary Martino, Becky French, Shelley Sharp, Lisa Durkin, UHN, Toronto Western Hospital, Toronto (Ont.) Trois établissements de soins de longue durée du Toronto West Regional Stroke Network (réseau régional de traitement des accidents vasculaires cérébraux de l’Ouest de Toronto) mettront sur pied des équipes de soins pour les survivants d’un accident vasculaire cérébral atteints de la dysphagie. La communication présentera les obstacles à franchir pour former ces équipes, les connaissances qui doit posséder le personnel et leur niveau de satisfaction. Ces équipes aideront à repérer les survivants qui doivent être suivis par un orthophoniste. 70 Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 Congrès de l’ACOA 2006 - Abrégés Mesures d’impact de l’aphasie : cadre pour « mesurer ce qui compte » Aura Kagan, Jennifer Hicks, Elyse Shumway, The Aphasia Institute, Toronto, ON; Nina Simmons-Mackie, Southeastern Louisiana University and The Aphasia Institute, Abita Springs, LA; Maria Huijbregts, Baycrest Centre for Geriatric Care, Toronto, ON; Sara McEwen, The Aphasia Institute, Hawkestone, ON Cette communication relèvera les écarts de la mesure des résultats entre la vraie participation à la vie et la vie avec l’aphasie. Les auteures présenteront un cadre convivial et accessible pour les aphasiologistes en milieu clinique qui saisit les grands concepts du savoir actuel au sein de notre secteur et au-delà et elles s’en serviront particulièrement pour mesurer les résultats de vie réelle des aphasiques ayant subi une intervention. L’aphasie et les soins en établissement Jennifer Sweeney, cabinet privé, Vancouver (C.-B.) Au cours des dernières années, l’exercice de l’orthophonie a changé pour inclure les principes de soins axés sur les clients et sur la famille. Nous disposons de connaissances et d’outils pour favoriser l’autonomie et la qualité de vie. Cette communication mettra l’accent sur les défis d’offrir un soutien dans tout le système aux aphasiques qui sont admis en établissement. Les besoins en matière de communication des adultes qui ne parlent pas et qui reçoivent des soins actifs: résultats d’une enquête Colleen A. Braun-Janzen, Leslie Mennell, Centre des sciences de la santé, Winnipeg (Man.) Cette communication présente une étude sur les modèles de pratique, les opinions concernant les normes des meilleures pratiques pour la prise en charge en soins actifs de patients qui ne parlent pas. Les résultats de l’enquête laissent entendre que le nombre d’orthophonistes dans les établissements de soins actifs est insuffisant pour répondre de manière adéquate aux besoins en matière de communication, ce qui fait que les infirmières doivent souvent faciliter la communication directe. Attitudes et perceptions des adultes aux prises avec des troubles acquis graves de la communication Colleen A. Braun-Janzen, Deer Lodge Centre, Winnipeg (Man.) Cette communication présente une enquête menée auprès de quatorze adultes aux prises avec un trouble acquis de la communication et qui doivent recourir à la communication suppléante et alternative. Les répondants ont réfléchi aux avantages et aux frustrations d’utiliser cette forme de communication, à la transition d’une personne qui parle et à une personne qui doit compter sur la communication suppléante et alternative, et à des suggestions pour aider les personnes qui parlent à communiquer avec les personnes qui utilisent la communication suppléante et alternative. Évaluation des carences de la communication cognitive de haut niveau Sheila MacDonald, Sheila MacDonald & Associates, Guelph (Ont.) L’évaluation d’un déficit léger de la communication après une lésion cérébrale acquise peut poser tout un défi pour l’orthophoniste en milieu clinique. L’évaluation fonctionnelle de la logique verbale et des stratégies d’exécution est un test uniforme conçu pour aider l’orthophoniste dans cette situation. Cette communication discutera des enjeux liés à l’évaluation des troubles cognitifs de la communication et présentera les résultats d’une étude normative de cette évaluation. Mesures des résultats obtenus par les enfants atteints du spectre autistique après que leurs parents aient suivi une formation Tara Davies, Luigi Girolametto, University of Toronto, Toronto (Ont.); Fern Sussman, Elaine Weitzman, The Hanen Centre, Toronto (Ont.) Cette communication résumera les résultats obtenus par les participants au programme More Than Words, qui s’adresse aux parents d’un enfant atteint du spectre autistique. À la suite du programme, les douze enfants ont réussi à accroître leur durée d’attention conjointe. Les résultats obtenus par les parents, tels qu’ils ont été mesurés par l’échelle Joy and Fun (JAFA; McConachie et Currill, 2002) montrent une amélioration du score final entre le test mené avant la formation et celui après. Initiation au traitement Relationship Development Intervention™ pour les personnes aux prises avec le spectre autistique Stephannie R. Motuz, Rehabilitation Centre for Children, Winnipeg (Man.) Cette communication définira les techniques de Relationship Development Intervention™ en passant en revue les modes de prestation de services et la démarche d’agrément pour les experts-conseils. Elle effectuera un bref survol des grandes lacunes qui seront corrigées et des techniques d’intervention avant de présenter deux études de cas. Elle décrira aussi l’étude locale en cours sur l’efficacité de ces techniques. Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 71 Congrès de l’ACOA 2006 - Abrégés Explorer la dimension éthique de l’orthophonie pédiatrique Eleanor Stewart, University of Alberta, Edmonton (Alb.) Cette communication présentera un compte rendu détaillé des pratiques en matière de déontologie adoptées par les orthophonistes qui travaillent auprès d’enfants. À partir d’une méthode fondée sur la théorie à base empirique, cette recherche s’est penchée sur le point de vue des cliniciens exerçant en milieu clinique. Le modèle qui en résulte a fait ressortir l’objectif moral central et a défini les actions en matière d’éthique et les caractéristiques d’un clinicien respectueux de la déontologie. Relever le défi de la formation clinique! Susan J. Wagner, University of Toronto, Toronto (Ont.); Lu-Anne McFarlane, University of Alberta, Edmonton (Alb.) Conférenciers à annoncer: Dalhousie University, Halifax (N.-É.), Université McGill, Montréal (Qué.), University of British Columbia, Vancouver (C.-B.), Université Laval, Québec (Qué.), Université de Montréal, Montréal (Qué.), Université d’Ottawa, Ottawa (Ont.) et University of Western Ontario, London (Ont.) La recherche et l’expérience montrent que les cliniciens reçoivent seulement une préparation minimale pour offrir une formation clinique fondée sur les meilleures pratiques aux étudiants. Ce panel d’experts composé de coordonnateurs de stages en milieu clinique a pour objectif d’examiner la formation clinique. Les participants pourront partager leurs stratégies fructueuses pour relever le défi de la formation clinique. Mon petit doigt me dit que nous ne sommes plus en séance de thérapie John Serkiz, ministère de la Santé et du Bien-être du Nouveau-Brunswick, Fredericton (N.-B.); Michele Lonergan, Régie régionale de la santé de Miramichi, Miramichi (N.-B.) Le Nouveau-Brunswick a mis en œuvre l’initiative Parle-moi/Talk With Me, qui favorise l’apprentissage de la communication chez tous les enfants d’âge préscolaire. Grâce à la promotion, à la prévention et à la mise en valeur du potentiel, ce service unique devrait avoir une incidence positive pour préparer les enfants à l’école dans toute la province. Recours à l’ENNI pour l’évaluation narrative ou l’élaboration de normes Phyllis Schneider, Denyse Hayward, University of Alberta, Edmonton (Alb.); Rita V. Dube, Toronto District School Board, Toronto (Ont.) Dans cette communication, nous présenterons un instrument pour recueillir de l’information sur les capacités à conter des histoires des enfants de 4 à 9 ans. Cet instrument porte le nom d’Edmonton Narrative Norms Instrument (ENNI). Nous décrirons comment l’ENNI sert à évaluer les capacités à conter des histoires des enfants. Les participants auront la possibilité d’en faire l’expérience pratique. Nous discuterons aussi de la façon d’élaborer des normes locales. COMMUNICATIONS AFFICHÉES ORTHOPHONIE Les aptitudes de communication et la maladie de Creutzfelt-Jakob : une vignette d’étude de cas Gopee Krishnan, Raj Shekar, Académie d’études supérieures de Manipal, Manipal, Karnataka, Inde La maladie de Creutzfelt-Jakob est un trouble mortel extrêmement rare à progression rapide. Elle a un phénotype clinique unique. Les aptitudes à la communication et différentes autres habiletés cognitives et motrices sont souvent atteintes. Ce rapport de cas tentera de présenter les caractéristiques cliniques, la pathophysiologie, les résultats d’évaluations neurologiques et cognitivo-linguistiques, et les stratégies de gestion de la maladie. FLUIDI TÉ FLUIDIT Indice de gravité subjective de l’effet de SpeechEasy® sur le bégaiement Marie S. Mossman, Cape Breton Family Place Resource Centre, Sydney (N.-É.); Tiffany M. Steeves, Berry Mills (N.B.); Joy Armson, Michael Kiefte, Dalhousie University, Halifax (N.-É.) Des participants ont évalué la gravité du bégaiement à partir d’échantillons de lecture orale prélevés avant que des personnes bègues portent l’appareil SpeechEasy® et après. La gravité du bégaiement décroît de 3,31 points sur une échelle de 9 points pour les personnes qui portent l’appareil par comparaison à celles qui ne le portent pas. Exception faite de deux cas, nous avons trouvé une relation linéaire entre l’évaluation de la gravité et la fréquence du bégaiement. DYSPHAGIE Nécessité d’une matrice de soins pour la dysphagie en milieu de soins actifs Genefer Behamdouni, St. Joseph’s Health Centre, Toronto (Ont.) Dans le passé, la priorité de chaque cas était fondée sur divers facteurs cliniques, émotifs et liés à des contraintes de temps. Cette communication présentera une approche uniforme pour la gestion des cas qui sert à prendre des décisions en clinique et à réduire les risques pour les patients. Elle traitera aussi d’une matrice de soins aux adultes. 72 Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 Congrès de l’ACOA 2006 - Abrégés Gestion de l’alimentation et de la déglutition: lignes directrices du Manitoba et politique de Winnipeg Shelley Irvine Day, Angela Forrest Kenning, Kelly Tye Vallis, Monique Piatt, Deer Lodge Centre, Winnipeg (Man.) En 2001, le ministère de la Santé du Manitoba a distribué le Manuel de Gestion des problèmes alimentaires et des troubles de déglutition dans les établissements de soins de longue durée à tout le personnel des établissements de soins de longue durée de la province. En 2004, l’Office régional de la santé de Winnipeg a transformé ces lignes directrices en une politique. Cette présentation par affiche soulignera la portée des énoncés contenus dans les deux documents, les incidences pour l’orthophonie et le processus de mise en œuvre à l’Office. Déglutition laborieuse par opposition à non-laborieuse : enquête sur les modèles de mouvement du larynx Veronique L. Philbin, Rebecca C. Smith, Syed Salman Qadri, Ruth E. Martin, University of Western Ontario, London (Ont.) Nous avons cherché à déterminer s’il était possible de distinguer la déglutition qui demande un effort de celle sans effort chez des sujets en santé grâce à un transducteur de mouvement laryngien. Les résultats montrent que chaque forme de déglutition produit des types très différents de mouvement laryngien. Ainsi, les techniques d’enregistrement et d’affichage du mouvement laryngien pourraient s’avérer être des approches de rétroaction biologique très utiles pour les patients atteints de trouble de la déglutition. Améliorer la réadaptation des personnes dysphagiques : l’ICF a-t-elle un rôle à jouer? Julie A. Theurer, University of Western Ontario, London (Ont.) Cette communication passera en revue les méthodes habituelles de traitement des troubles de déglutition et examinera les questions soulevées dans des articles récents qui mettent en lumière la nécessité d’adopter de nouvelles pratiques novatrices en clinique et en recherche. Elle explorera aussi le rôle de la Classification internationale sur le fonctionnement, l’invalidité et la santé en vue d’améliorer le traitement de la dysphagie. LANGAGE DE L’ENFANT Efficacité de la sensibilisation phonologique chez les enfants atteints du syndrome de Down Patricia L. Cleave, Elizabeth Kay-Raining Bird, Katie MacIsaac, Melissa Armstrong, Dalhousie University, Halifax (N.É.); Derrick C. Bourassa, Acadia University, Wolfville (N.-É.) Cette présentation par affiche décrira les résultats d’un programme de formation conçu pour faciliter la sensibilisation phonologique chez les enfants atteints du syndrome de Down. Huit enfants ont participé à un programme de vingtdeux semaines. Leur réaction à l’intervention a varié; quatre des huit enfants ont montré une amélioration. Les variables permettant de prévoir le succès de l’intervention seront présentées. « Let’s Read and Talk » : un programme communautaire préscolaire pour apprendre à parler, à lire et à écrire Janet P. Simpson, Office régional de la santé de Winnipeg, Winnipeg (Man.) Cette communication décrira un programme communautaire de promotion de la santé conçu pour améliorer les compétences des intervenants afin qu’ils puissent faciliter l’apprentissage du langage ainsi que l’éveil à la lecture et à l’écriture chez les enfants d’âge préscolaire. À partir d’une histoire dans un livre, on a présenté les meilleures pratiques et on a encadré leur mise en œuvre. Une évaluation et des enregistrements effectués avant et après le programme ont montré des améliorations importantes et significatives. Traitement assisté par ordinateur : effets et utilisation de l’orthophonie Karla N. Washington, Genese A. Warr-Leeper, University of Western Ontario, London (Ont.) Les résultats avant et après deux programmes de traitement assisté par ordinateur chez des enfants d’âge préscolaire ont été examinés. Un programme visait la phonologie et l’autre, la langue. Le programme sur la phonologie a eu pour effet d’accroître l’exactitude et l’intelligibilité. L’autre programme a réussi à améliorer l’exactitude ainsi que la variété, la longueur et la complexité des structures de phrases. Densité de la reformulation et acquisition des temps de verbe irréguliers du passé Kerry E. Proctor-Williams, East Tennessee State University, Johnson City (Tennessee); Marc E. Fey, University of Kansas Medical Center, Kansas City (Kansas) Center, Kansas City, KS À un débit de reformulation en conversation, treize enfants ayant un langage type ont utilisé des verbes irréguliers au passé avec davantage d’exactitude que des enfants atteints d’un trouble d’orthophonie. À un débit de reformulation en cours de traitement, l’exactitude des verbes ne s’est pas améliorée chez les enfants atteints d’un trouble et elle a diminué chez les autres enfants. Les productions métalinguistiques des enfants semblent indiquer que les groupes ont utilisé l’information de reformulation de manière différente. Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 73 Congrès de l’ACOA 2006 - Abrégés SERVICES POUR ADULTES Analy se de la ssynt ynt ax e chez les aphasiques de Br oca et de W ernick e Analyse yntax axe Broca Wernick ernicke Fouzia Badaoui, CRSTDLA, Alger Nous présentons dans cet article les résultats d’une recherche portant sur l’analyse de la syntaxe chez deux aphasiques pour déduire son fonctionnement en appliquant le modèle linguistique néo-khalilien. Incidence de l’orthophonie dans les cas d’aphasie primaire progressive avec communication non fluide Annie Delyfer, Fanny Singer, CRIR, Hôpital juif de réadaptation, Laval (Qué.); Eva Kehayia, Université McGill et CRIR, Montréal (Qué.); Nancy Azevedo, Université McGill, Montréal (Qué.) Cette présentation par affiche traitera de l’utilité de l’orthophonie conventionnelle auprès de patients atteints d’aphasie primaire progressive avec communication non fluide. Une analyse documentaire de même que les résultats de tests effectués chez l’un de ces patients à trois moments différents indiquent que l’orthophonie peut améliorer certaines fonctions du langage pendant un certain temps. Aphasie croisée et capacité de communication gestuelle Patrick Coppens, Plattsburgh State University of New York, Plattsburgh (New York); Sylvie Hébert, Université de Montréal, Montréal (Qué.); Lise Gagnon, Université de Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke (Qué.) L’aphasie croisée résulte d’une lésion de l’hémisphère droit chez un droitier. Ce type de latéralisation inhabituelle de la langue influence-t-il l’organisation neurologique des compétences dans l’hémisphère droit, comme la capacité de chanter? À partir d’un cas clinique, cette présentation par affiche s’attardera à cette question et fera des extrapolations pour la réadaptation des personnes aphasiques. Élaboration d’un programme interdisciplinaire de réadaptation fondé sur la faible tolérance aux longues durées pour les personnes ayant subi un accident vasculaire cérébral Lauren Murphy, Fabian Krupski, St. Joseph’s Health Centre, Toronto (Ont.) Les besoins en réadaptation des survivants d’un accident vasculaire cérébral ne sont pas homogènes. Cette communication se penchera sur l’élaboration d’un programme interdisciplinaire de réadaptation fondé sur la faible tolérance aux longues durées dans un établissement de soins prolongés. Ce programme est conçu spécialement pour les personnes qui, en raison de leur âge ou de la gravité de leur accident vasculaire cérébral, ont besoin de services plus spécialisés. Rôle de la phonologie dans la reconnaissance des mots : une comparaison de lecteurs de niveau collégial Randeep K. Sidhu, University of North Dakota, Winnipeg (Man.); Charlene Chamberlain, University of North Dakota, Grand Forks (North Dakota) Des étudiants de premier cycle (11 qui sont bons en lecture, 11 qui sont moyens et 9 qui sont faibles) ont passé un test de lecture et ont effectué une tâche de décision lexicale et de dénomination à partir de stimulus quasi-homophones. Cet exercice visait à examiner le rôle de la phonologie pour la reconnaissance de mots nécessaire à la lecture. Les résultats montrent qu’il n’y a pas d’interaction. Cette communication abordera les conséquences en milieu clinique. EXERCICE CLINIQUE Intervention axée sur les compétences : à quoi cela ressemblerait-il? Carolyn Cronk, Julie Fortier-Blanc, Université de Montréal, Montréal (Qué.) Cette communication explorera l’incidence des services d’orthophonie comme moyen d’établir une série de compétences ou de capacités fonctionnelles construites à partir d’une combinaison de connaissances accumulées ou de sensibilité, d’aptitudes de base et d’habiletés apprises. Plusieurs cas tirés du milieu clinique serviront d’exemples. L’orthophonie va de l’avant avec les assistants numériques Alexa Okrainec, Brandon University, Brandon (Man.) L’assistant numérique, qui est un puissant ordinateur de poche, est une nouveauté qui peut faire progresser la prestation de services d’orthophonie. Cette communication mettra en valeur l’utilisation de cet outil en pratique clinique, qui dépasse les fonctions bien connues de carnet d’adresse et d’agenda. 74 Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 Information for Contributors The Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology (JSLPA) welcomes submissions of scholarly manuscripts related to human communication and its disorders broadly defined. This includes submissions relating to normal and disordered processes of speech, language, and hearing. Manuscripts that have not been published previously are invited in English and French. Manuscripts may be tutorial, theoretical, integrative, practical, pedagogic, or empirical. All manuscripts will be evaluated on the basis of the timeliness, importance, and applicability of the submission to the interests of speech–language pathology and audiology as professions, and to communication sciences and disorders as a discipline. Consequently, all manuscripts are assessed in relation to the potential impact of the work on improving our understanding of human communication and its disorders. All categories of manuscripts submitted will undergo peer-review to determine the suitability of the submission for publication in JSLPA. The Journal recently has established multiple categories of manuscript submission that will permit the broadest opportunity for disseminaion of information related to human communication and its disorders. New categories for manuscript submission include: Tutorials. Review articles, treatises, or position papers that address a specific topic within either a theoretical or clinical framework. Articles. Traditional manuscripts addressing applied or basic experimental research on issues related to speech, language, and/or hearing with human participants or animals. Clinical Reports. Reports of new clinical procedures, protocols, or methods with specific focus on direct application to identification, assessment and/or treatment concerns in speech, language, and/or hearing. Brief Reports. Similar to research notes, brief communications concerning preliminary findings, either clinical or experimental (applied or basic), that may lead to additional and more comprehensive study in the future. These reports are typically based on small “n” or pilot studies and must address disordered participant populations. Research Notes. Brief communications that focus on experimental work conducted in laboratory settings. These reports will typically address methodological concerns and/or modifications of existing tools or instruments with either normal or disordered populations. Field Reports. Reports that outline the provision of services that are conducted in unique, atypical, or nonstandard settings; manuscripts in this category may include screening, assessment, and/or treatment reports. Letters to the Editor. A forum for presentation of scholarly/ clinical differences of opinion concerning work previously published in the Journal. Letters to the Editor may influence our thinking about design considerations, methodological confounds, data analysis and/or data interpretation, etc. As with other categories of submissions, this communication forum is contingent upon peer-review. However, in contrast to other categories of submission, rebuttal from the author(s) will be solicited upon acceptance of a letter to the editor. Submission of Manuscripts Contributors should send five (5) copies of manuscripts including all tables, figures or illustrations, and references to: Phyllis Schneider, PhD Editor, JSLPA Dept. of Speech Pathology and Audiology University of Alberta 2-70 Corbett Hall Edmonton, AB T6G 2G4 Along with copies of the manuscript, a cover letter indicating that the manuscript is being submitted for publication consideration should be included. The cover letter must explicitly state that the manuscript is original work, that has not been published previously, and that it is not currently under review elsewhere. Manuscripts are received and peer-reviewed contingent upon this understanding. The author(s) must also provide appropriate confirmation that work conducted with humans or animals has received ethical review and approval. Failure to provide information on ethical approval will delay the review process. Finally, the cover letter should also indicate the category of submission (i.e., tutorial, clinical report, etc.). If the editorial staff determines that the manuscript should be considered within another category, the contact author will be notified. All submissions should conform to the publication guidelines of the Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association (APA), 5th Edition. Manuscripts should be word processed, IBM format preferred. Should the manuscript be accepted for publication, submission of a diskette version of the submission will facilitate the publication process. A confirmation of receipt for all manuscripts will be provided to the contact author prior to distribution for peer-review. JSLPA seeks to conduct the review process and respond to authors regarding the outcome of the review within 90 days of receipt. If a manuscript is judged as suitable for publication in JSLPA, authors will have 30 days to make necessary revisions prior to a secondary review. The author is responsible for all statements made in his or her manuscript, including changes made by the editorial and/ or production staff. Upon final acceptance of a manuscript and immediately prior to publication, the contact author will be permitted to review galley proofs and verify its content to the publication office within 72 hours of receipt of galley proofs. Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 75 Organization of the Manuscript 76 All copies should be typed, double-spaced, with a standard typeface (12 point, noncompressed font) on high quality 8 ½ X 11 paper. All margins should be at least one (1) inch. An original and four (copies) of the manuscript should be submitted directly to the Editor. Author identification for the review process is optional; if blind-review is desired, three (3) of the copies should be prepared accordingly (cover page and acknowledgments blinded). Responsibility for removing all potential identifying information rests solely with the author(s). All manuscripts should be prepared according to APA guidelines. This manual is available from most university bookstores or is accessible via commercial bookstores. Generally, the following sections should be submitted in the order specified. Title Page: This page should include the full title of the manuscript, the full names of the author(s) with academic degrees, each author’s affiliation, and a complete mailing address for the contact author. An electronic mail address also is recommended. Abstract Abstract: On a separate sheet of paper, a brief yet informative abstract that does not exceed one page is required. The abstract should include the purpose of the work along with pertinent information relative to the specific manuscript category for which it was submitted. Key Words: Following the abstract and on the same page, the author(s) should supply a list of key words for indexing purposes. Tables: Eachtableincludedinthemanuscriptmustbetypewritten and double-spaced on a separate sheet of paper. Tables should be numbered consecutively beginning with Table 1. Each table must have a descriptive caption. Tables should serve to expand the information provided in the text of the manuscript, not to duplicate information. Illustrations: All illustrations included as part of the manuscript will need to be included with each copy of the manuscript. While a single copy of original artwork (black and white photographs, x-ray films, etc.) is required, all manuscripts must have clear copies of all illustrations for the review process. For photographs, 5 x 7 glossy prints are preferred. High quality laser printed materials are also acceptable. For other types of computerized illustrations, it is recommended that JSLPA production staff be consulted prior to preparation and submission of the manuscript and associated figures/illustrations. Legends for Illustrations: Legends for all figures and illustrations should be typewritten (double-spaced) on a separate sheet of paper with numbers corresponding to the order in which figures/ illustrations appear in the manuscript. Page Numbering and Running Head: Thetextofthemanuscript should be prepared with each page numbered, including tables, figures/illustrations, references, and if appropriate, appendices. A short (30 characters or less) descriptive running title should appear at the top right hand margin of each page of the manuscript. Acknowledgments: Acknowledgments should be typewritten (double-spaced) on a separate sheet of paper. Appropriate acknowledgment for any type of sponsorship, donations, grants, technical assistance, and to professional colleagues who contributed to the work, but are not listed as authors, should be noted. References: Referencesaretobelistedconsecutivelyinalphabetical order, then chronologically for each author. Authors should consult the APA publication manual (4th Edition) for methods of citing varied sources of information. Journal names and appropriate volume number should be spelled out and italicized. All literature, tests and assessment tools, and standards (ANSI and ISO) must be listed in the references. All references should be double-spaced. Potential Conflicts of Interest and Dual Commitment Participants in Research Humans and Animals As part of the submission process, the author(s) must explicitly identify if any potential conflict of interest, or dual commitment, exists relative to the manuscript and its author(s). Such disclosure is requested so as to inform JSLPA that the author or authors have the potential to benefit from publication of the manuscript. Such benefits may be either direct or indirect and may involve financial and/or other nonfinancial benefit(s) to the author(s). Disclosure of potential conflicts of interest or dual commitment may be provided to editorial consultants if it is believed that such a conflict of interest or dual commitment may have had the potential to influence the information provided in the submission or compromise the design, conduct, data collection or analysis, and/or interpretation of the data obtained and reported in the manuscript submitted for review. If the manuscript is accepted for publication, editorial acknowledgement of such potential conflict of interest or dual commitment may occur when publication occurs. Each manuscript submitted to JSLPA for peer-review that is based on work conducted with humans or animals must acknowledge appropriate ethical approval. In instances where humans or animals have been used for research, a statement indicating that the research was approved by an institutional review board or other appropriate ethical evaluation body or agency must clearly appear along with the name and affiliation of the research ethics and the ethical approval number. The review process will not begin until this information is formally provided to the Editor. Similar to research involving human participants, JSLPA requires that work conducted with animals state that such work has met with ethical evaluation and approval. This includes identification of the name and affiliation of the research ethics evaluation body or agency and the ethical approval number. A statement that all research animals were used and cared for in an established and ethically approved manner is also required. The review process will not begin until this information is formally provided to the Editor. Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 Renseignements à l’intention des collaborateurs La Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie (ROA) est heureuse de se voir soumettre des manuscrits de recherche portant sur la communication humaine et sur les troubles qui s’y rapportent, dans leur sens large. Cela comprend les manuscrits portant sur les processus normaux et désordonnés de la parole, du langage et de l’audition. Nous recherchons des manuscrits qui n’ont jamais été publiés, en français ou en anglais. Les manuscrits peuvent être tutoriels, théoriques, synthétiques, pratiques, pédagogiques ou empiriques. Tous les manuscrits seront évalués en fonction de leur signification, de leur opportunité et de leur applicabilité aux intérêts de l’orthophonie et de l’audiologie comme professions, et aux sciences et aux troubles de la communication en tant que disciplines. Par conséquent, tous les manuscrits sont évalués en fonction de leur incidence possible sur l’amélioration de notre compréhension de la communication humaine et des troubles qui s’y rapportent. Peu importe la catégorie, tous les manuscrits présentés seront soumis à une révision par des collègues afin de déterminer s’ils peuvent être publiés dans la ROA. La Revue a récemment établi plusieurs catégories de manuscrits afin de permettre la meilleure diffusion possible de l’information portant sur la communication humaine et les troubles s’y rapportant. Les nouvelles catégories de manuscrits comprennent : Tutoriels : Rapports de synthèse, traités ou exposés de position portant sur un sujet particulier dans un cadre théorique ou clinique. Articles : Manuscrits conventionnels traitant de recherche appliquée ou expérimentale de base sur les questions se rapportant à la parole, au langage ou à l’audition et faisant intervenir des participants humains ou animaux. Comptes rendus cliniques : Comptes rendus de nouvelles procédures ou méthodes ou de nouveaux protocoles cliniques portant particulièrement sur une application directe par rapport aux questions d’identification, d’évaluation et de traitement relativement à la parole, au langage et à l’audition. Comptes rendus sommaires : Semblables aux notes de recherche, brèves communications portant sur des conclusions préliminaires, soit cliniques soit expérimentales (appliquées ou fondamentales), pouvant mener à une étude plus poussée dans l’avenir. Ces comptes rendus se fondent typiquement sur des études à petit « n » ou pilotes et doivent traiter de populations désordonnées. Notes de recherche : Brèves communications traitant spécifiquement de travaux expérimentaux menés en laboratoire. Ces comptes rendus portent typiquement sur des questions de méthodologie ou des modifications apportées à des outils existants utilisés auprès de populations normales ou désordonnées. Comptes rendus d’expérience : Comptes rendus décrivant sommairement la prestation de services offerts en situations uniques, atypiques ou particulières; les manuscrits de cette catégorie peuvent comprendre des comptes rendus de dépistage, d’évaluation ou de traitement. Courrier des lecteurs : Forum de présentation de divergences de vues scientifiques ou cliniques concernant des ouvrages déjà publiés dans la Revue. Le Courrier des lecteurs peut avoir un effet sur notre façon de penser par rapport aux facteurs de conception, aux confusions méthodologiques, à l’analyse ou l’interprétation des données, etc. Comme c’est le cas pour d’autres catégories de présentation, ce forum de communication est soumis à une révision par des collègues. Cependant, contrairement aux autres catégories, on recherchera la réaction des auteurs sur acceptation d’une lettre. Présentation de manuscrits On demande aux collaborateurs de faire parvenir cinq (5) exemplaires de leurs manuscrits, y compris tous les tableaux, figures ou illustrations et références, à : Phyllis Schneider, Ph.D. Rédactrice en chef, Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie Dept. of Speech Pathology and Audiology University of Alberta 2-70 Corbett Hall Edmonton (Alberta) T6G 2G4 On doit joindre aux exemplaires du manuscrit une lettre d’envoi qui indiquera que le manuscrit est présenté en vue de sa publication. La lettre d’envoi doit préciser que le manuscrit est une œuvre originale, qu’il n’a pas déjà été publié et qu’il ne fait pas actuellement l’objet d’un autre examen en vue d’être publié. Les manuscrits sont reçus et examinés sur acceptation de ces conditions. L’auteur (les auteurs) doit (doivent) aussi fournir une attestation en bonne et due forme que toute recherche impliquant des êtres humains ou des animaux a fait l’objet de l’agrément d’un comité de révision déontologique. L’absence d’un tel agrément retardera le processus de révision. Enfin, la lettre d’envoi doit également préciser la catégorie de la présentation (i.e. tutoriel, rapport clinique, etc.). Si l’équipe d’examen juge que le manuscrit devrait passer sous une autre catégorie, l’auteur-contact en sera avisé. Toutes les présentations doivent se conformer aux lignes de conduite présentées dans le Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association (APA), 5th Edition. Les manuscrits doivent être dactylographiés sur traitement de texte en format IBM, de préférence. L’envoi d’une disquette, si le manuscrit est accepté, facilite la publication. Un accusé de réception de chaque manuscrit sera envoyé à l’auteur-contact avant la distribution des exemplaires en vue de la révision. La ROA cherche à effectuer cette révision et à informer les auteurs des résultats de cette révision dans les 90 jours de la réception. Lorsqu’on juge que le manuscrit convient à la ROA, on donnera 30 jours aux auteurs pour effectuer les changements nécessaires avant l’examen secondaire. L’auteur est responsable de toutes les affirmations formulées dans son manuscrit, y compris toutes les modifications effectuées par les rédacteurs et réviseurs. Sur acceptation définitive du manuscrit et immédiatement avant sa publication, on donnera l’occasion à l’auteur-contact de revoir les épreuves et il devra signifier la vérification du contenu dans les 72 heures suivant réception de ces épreuves. Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 77 Organisation du manuscrit Tous les textes doivent être dactylographiés à double interligne, en caractère standard (police de caractères 12 points, non comprimée) et sur papier 8 ½” X 11" de qualité. Toutes les marges doivent être d’au moins un (1) pouce. L’original et quatre (4) copies du manuscrit doiventêtreprésentésdirectementaurédacteurenchef.L’identification de l’auteur est facultative pour le processus d’examen : si l’auteur souhaite ne pas être identifié à ce stade, il devra préparer trois (3) copies d’un manuscrit dont la page couverture et les remerciements seront voilés. Seuls les auteurs sont responsables de retirer toute information identificatrice éventuelle. Tous les manuscrits doivent être rédigés en conformité aux lignes de conduite de l’APA. Ce manuel est disponible dans la plupart des librairies universitaires et peut être commandé chez les libraires commerciaux. En général, les sections qui suivent doivent être présentées dans l’ordre chronologique précisé. Page titre : Cette page doit contenir le titre complet du manuscrit, les noms complets des auteurs, y compris les diplômes et affiliations, et l’adresse complète de l’auteur-contact. Une adresse de courriel est également recommandée. Abrégé : Sur une page distincte, produire un abrégé bref mais informateur ne dépassant pas une page. L’abrégé doit indiquer l’objet du travail ainsi que toute information pertinente portant sur la catégorie du manuscrit. Mots clés : Immédiatement suivant l’abrégé et sur la même page, les auteurs doivent présenter une liste de mots clés aux fins de constitution d’un index. Tableaux : Tous les tableaux compris dans un même manuscrit doivent être dactylographiés à double interligne sur une page distincte. Les tableaux doivent être numérotés consécutivement, en commençant par le Tableau 1. Chaque tableau doit être accompagné d’une légende et doit servir à compléter les renseignements fournis dans le texte du manuscrit plutôt qu’à reprendre l’information contenue dans le texte ou dans les tableaux. Illustrations : Toutes les illustrations faisant partie du manuscrit doiventêtreinclusesavecchaqueexemplairedumanuscrit.Quoiqu’un Conflits d’intérêts possibles et engagement double Dans le processus de présentation, les auteurs doivent déclarer clairementl’existencedetoutconflitd’intérêtspossiblesouengagement double relativement au manuscrit et de ses auteurs. Cette déclaration est nécessaire afin d’informer la ROA que l’auteur ou les auteurs peuvent tirer avantage de la publication du manuscrit. Ces avantages pour les auteurs, directs ou indirects, peuvent être de nature financière ou non financière. La déclaration de conflit d’intérêts possibles ou d’engagement double peut être transmise à des conseillers en matière de publication lorsqu’on estime qu’un tel conflit d’intérêts ou engagement double aurait pu influencer l’information fournie dans la présentation ou compromettre la conception, la conduite, la collecte ou l’analyse des données, ou l’interprétation des données recueillies et présentées dans le manuscrit soumis à l’examen. Si le manuscrit est accepté en vue de sa publication, la rédaction se réserve le droit de reconnaître l’existence possible d’un tel conflit d’intérêts ou engagement double. 78 seul exemplaire du matériel d’illustration original (photographies, radiographies, etc.) soit requis, chaque manuscrit doit contenir des copies claires de toutes les illustrations pour le processus de révision. Dans le cas de photographies, on préfère les photos sur papier glacé 5" X 7". Les impressions au laser de haute qualité sont acceptables. Pour les autres types d’illustrations informatisées, il est recommandé de consulter le personnel de production de la ROA avant la préparation et la présentation du manuscrit et des figures et illustrations s’y rattachant. Légendes des illustrations : Les légendes accompagnant chaque figure et illustration doivent être dactylographiées à double interligne sur une feuille distincte et identifiées à l’aide d’un numéro qui correspond à la séquence de parution des figures et illustrations dans le manuscrit. Numérotation des pages et titre courant : Chaque page du manuscrit doit être numérotée, y compris les tableaux, figures, illustrations, références et, le cas échéant, les annexes. Un bref (30 caractères ou moins) titre courant descriptif doit apparaître dans la marge supérieure droite de chaque page du manuscrit. Remerciements : Les remerciements doivent être dactylographiés à double interligne sur une feuille distincte. L’auteur doit reconnaître toute forme de parrainage, don, bourse ou d’aide technique, ainsi que tout collègue professionnel qui ont contribué à l’ouvrage mais qui n’est pas cité à titre d’auteur. Références : Les références sont énumérées les unes après les autres, en ordre alphabétique, suivi de l’ordre chronologique sous le nom de chaque auteur. Les auteurs doivent consulter le manuel de l’APA (5e Édition) pour obtenir la façon exacte de rédiger une citation. Les noms de revues scientifiques et autres doivent être rédigés au long et imprimés en italiques. Tous les ouvrages, outils d’essais et d’évaluation ainsi que les normes (ANSI et ISO) doivent figurer dans la liste de références. Les références doivent être dactylographiées à double interligne. Participants à la recherche – êtres humains et animaux Chaque manuscrit présenté à la ROA en vue d’un examen par des pairs et qui se fonde sur une recherche effectuée avec la participation d’être humains ou d’animaux doit faire état d’un agrément déontologique approprié. Dans les cas où des êtres humains ou des animaux ont servi à des fins de recherche, on doit joindre une attestation indiquant que la recherche a été approuvée par un comité d’examen reconnu ou par tout autre organisme d’évaluation déontologique, comportant le nom et l’affiliation de l’éthique de recherche ainsi que le numéro de l’approbation. Le processus d’examen ne sera pas amorcé avant que cette information ne soit formellement fournie au rédacteur en chef. Tout comme pour la recherche effectuée avec la participation d’êtres humains, la ROA exige que toute recherche effectuée avec des animaux soit accompagnée d’une attestation à l’effet que cette recherche a été évaluée et approuvée par les autorités déontologiques compétentes. Cela comporte le nom et l’affiliation de l’organisme d’évaluation de l’éthique en recherche ainsi que le numéro de l’approbation correspondante. On exige également une attestation à l’effet que tous les animaux de recherche ont été utilisés et soignés d’une manière reconnue et éthique. Le processus d’examen ne sera pas amorcé avant que cette information ne soit formellement fournie au rédacteur en chef. Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 APPEL POUR COMMUNICATIONS Congrès de l’ACOA 2007 Moncton, Nouveau-Brunswick du 25 - 28 avril, 2007 Date limite de réception des propositions: le 1er septembre 2006 Vous pouvez soumettre votre proposition de communication en ligne au: www.caslpa.ca/francais/events/conference.asp Le congrès annuel 2007 de l’Association canadienne des orthophonistes et audiologistes (ACOA) se tiendra à Moncton (Nouveau-Brunswick). L’ACOA vous invite donc à soumettre vos propositions de communication pour son programme du congrès annuel 2007. Les cliniciens de tous genres de pratique sont encouragés à partager leurs réflexions, leurs expériences, leurs méthodes et leurs recherches. L’ACOA souhaite recevoir des propositions de communications, de communications affichées, d’expositions scientifiques, de mini-séminaires de formation et de vidéocassettes. Les présentations multidisciplinaires seront également prises en considération. Les sessions se tiendront pendant le jour, du 25 au 28 avril, 2007. TYPES DE SESSION Présentation de communication communication: Une présentation de communication devrait être basée sur une recherche courante, une expérience clinique ou sur une étude de cas, être récente et ne pas avoir été publiée (durée de 45 minutes). Mini-séminaires Mini-séminaires: Ces séances sont conçues de manière à susciter des discussions interactives au sujet de la pratique clinique et des problèmes professionnels (durée de 90 minutes). Séances d’affichage: La présentation des affiches doit suffire, à elle seule, à fournir de l’information. Chaque présentoir doit contenir le titre et le nom du ou des auteurs, l’énoncé de principe, la méthodologie, les résultats et conclusions. Les affiches doivent être présentées sous format en largeur et selon des dimensions ne dépassant pas 2.4m par 1.2m. Lors de périodes établies à l’avance, les auteurs devront être présents pour répondre aux questions et participer aux échanges (discussions). Expositions scientifiques: Ces activités seront incorporées aux sessions d’affichage. Lors de périodes établies à l’avance, les exposants devront être présents pour décrire et discuter de leur exposition. Une table mesurant approximativement 1.8 m par .75 m et un tableau d’affichage de 2.4 m x 1.2 m seront mis à la disposition des exposants. Les exposants doivent fournir tout autre équipement nécessaire. Présentations de vidéocassette: Les vidéocassettes peuvent présenter des sujets cliniques, des études de cas, des agences, programmes, procédures de thérapie ou autres. Les vidéocassettes doivent être de type VHS (1/2 pouce). Évaluation et mise en oeuvre de nouvelles technologies/méthodes Mesure de performance ou de rendement (outcome) et efficacité Ce qui fonctionne en pratique/ conseils à suivre en milieu clinique Les services aux clientèles difficiles Formation de médiateurs/facilitateurs Éthique en milieu clinique Le formulaire pour soumettre les propositions de Effets du multiculturalisme communications, les conditions et les instructions peuvent être Modèles de prestation de services téléchar gés à partir du site W eb de l’ ACO A au www .caslpa.ca/ téléchargés Web l’A COA www.caslpa.ca/ Situations de transition (p. ex.: préscolairefr ancais/e v ent s/confer e ence .asp . V ous pouv ez soumettr e vvotr otr francais/e ancais/ev ents/confer s/conference ence.asp Vous pouvez soumettre otre scolaire, soins intensifs-communauté) demande en ligne ou en communiquant avec [email protected] pour La planification et la réalisation de recherche en obtenir un formulaire et informations par envoi postal ou milieu clinique. électr onique ou par télécopieur électronique télécopieur.. Autre Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie - Vol. 30, No 1, Printemps 2006 79 CALL FOR PAPERS CASLPA Conference 2006 Moncton, New Brunswick April 25 - 28, 2007 Deadline for receipt of all program submissions: September 1, 2006 Online abstract submissions at: www.caslpa.ca/english/events/conference.asp The Canadian Association of Speech-Language Pathologists and Audiologists (CASLPA) 2007 conference will be held in Moncton, New Brunswick. CASLPA invites program submissions to the annual conference. Clinicians from all practice settings are encouraged to share their insight, experience, methods and research. CASLPA invites submissions of papers, poster sessions, scientific exhibits, mini-seminars and videotapes. Multidisciplinary presentations will be considered. Sessions will be scheduled daily from April 25 - 28, 2007. SESSION TYPES Paper Presentations: A paper presentation should be based on current research that has not been published, clinical experience, or case studies (45 minutes in duration). Mini-seminars: These sessions are designed to provide opportunity for interactive discussion of clinical practice and professional issues (90 minutes in duration). Poster Sessions: Poster presentations should stand alone in conveying information. Each display should contain title and author(s), statement of purpose, methodology, results and conclusions. Posters must be in landscape format, no larger than 2.4 m x 1.2 m. Authors are required to be present at designated times to respond to questions and discussion. Scientific Exhibits: These sessions will be incorporated with the Poster Presentations. Exhibitors are required to be present at designated times to describe and discuss the exhibit. A table of approximately 1.8 m x .75 m and a poster board of approximately 2.4 m x1.2 m will be available. Exhibitors are responsible for providing all equipment that will be required. Videotape Presentations: Videotapes may be presented on clinical topics, case studies, agencies, therapy procedures or other topics. Videotapes must be on 1/2-inch VHS video cassette. 80 Evaluating and implementing new technologies/methods Measuring outcome and efficacy Best practice/clinical guidelines Hard-to-serve populations Mediator/facilitator training The complete Call ffor or P aper or Paper aperss including Conditions ffor Ethics in clinical practice Acceptance, Instructions and Request for Presentation form, can Multicultural considerations be do wnloaded fr om our w ebsite at: www .caslpa.ca/english/ downloaded from website www.caslpa.ca/english/ Service delivery models event s/confer ence .asp Y ou can submit on-line or cont act ents/confer s/conference ence.asp You contact Transition issues [email protected] to have a hard copy emailed, faxed or mailed to Designing and implementing clinical research you. Other Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology - Vol. 30, No. 1, Spring 2006 AUDIOLOGIST 1 PERMANENT or LOCUM Stephenville, Newfoundland and Labrador Western Regional Integrated Health Authority has a career opportunity for an Audiologist on the beautiful, scenic west coast of Newfoundland and Labrador, Canada. We offer our employees a unique wellness program, flexible working arrangements, excellent benefits package and a strong leadership team. To find out why we are one of the most desirable regions of Canada in which to live and work, log on to http://www.hcsw.nf.ca/arealinks.htm. This position offers an excellent opportunity to apply and develop skills while providing a variety of audiological services to persons of all ages with concerns of hearing and auditory function. The incumbent would work as a member of an interdisciplinary team. Included in the responsibilities of the position are diagnostic assessment and rehabilitative services, dispensing for the Provincial Hearing Aid Plan, public and professional education, consulting, regional clinics, and participation in collaborative community/professional partnerships which serve our clients and communities. The incumbent is also responsible for the supervision of an Audiology Technician. This position reports to the Regional Clinical Leader of Audiology located in Corner Brook. QUALIFICATIONS: A Master’s Degree in Audiology from a recognized university and eligibility for CASLPA certification. Monthly regional travel is required. A valid driver’s license and use of a private vehicle are also required. SALARY SCALE: $53,644.50 to $63,219.00 per Annum A $10,000 bursary is available to a new graduate in return for a service commitment of two years. Reimbursement of relocation expenses will be considered. We offer an attractive benefit package which includes four weeks annual vacation, nine statutory holidays, pension plan, health insurance, dental and long-term disability plans. Please Send Resume, Proof of Qualifications and the Names of Two References to: Donna Foss Recruitment & Orientation Coordinator Western Regional Integrated Health Authority P. O. Box 156, Corner Brook, NL A2H 6C7 Fax: (709) 637-5155 E-Mail: [email protected]; Website: www.hcsw.nf.ca PLEASE QUOTE ORDER NO. WH-06-93 WHEN APPLYING. Just another reason we’re continuing to raise the bar. To save a life, to get closer to a cure, to teach, to learn, to give someone a second chance and to make an impact… T his is why you chose a health care career. But to make the greatest impact, you need the best tools, the best resources and the strength of a dedicated team on your side. With a reputation for innovation and excellence, Capital Health has been recognized as a leading health system for five consecutive years by the Canadian Institute for Health Information. Affiliated with the University of Alberta, Capital Health is Canada’s largest academic health care region. Our staff enjoys a vibrant and diverse setting, a strong local economy, high calibre training and, most importantly, the opportunity to raise the bar. Enjoy the challenges and rewards offered by a leader in health care. Visit our website at www.capitalhealth.ca Talk to us today! Relocation assistance is available Capital Health Regional Recruitment Toll-free: 1-877-488-4860 • Local: 735-0124 E-mail: [email protected] People who care. Work that matters. We welcome all inquiries and thank you for your interest. 38271rcap.indd 1 2/3/2006 10:18:27 AM
Documents pareils
Revue canadienne d`orthophonie et d`audiologie
Measure Listening Effort’ and emphasizes the importance of adding measures of listening effort to clinical audiology.
We have two book reviews in this issue of the Canadian Journal of Speech-Langua...